Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Specialty Sites
    • COVID-19
    • Practice Current
    • Practice Buzz
    • Without Borders
    • Equity, Diversity and Inclusion
    • Innovations in Care Delivery
  • Collections
    • Topics A-Z
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Infographics
    • Patient Pages
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Author Center

Advanced Search

Main menu

  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Specialty Sites
    • COVID-19
    • Practice Current
    • Practice Buzz
    • Without Borders
    • Equity, Diversity and Inclusion
    • Innovations in Care Delivery
  • Collections
    • Topics A-Z
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Infographics
    • Patient Pages
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Author Center
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Residents & Fellows

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Neurology
Home
The most widely read and highly cited peer-reviewed neurology journal
  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
Site Logo
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Residents & Fellows

Share

January 01, 1995; 45 (1) Articles

Subcortical aphasia

Distinct profiles following left putaminal hemorrhage

Mark D'Esposito, Michael P. Alexander
First published January 1, 1995, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.45.1.38
Mark D'Esposito
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michael P. Alexander
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Full PDF
Citation
Subcortical aphasia
Distinct profiles following left putaminal hemorrhage
Mark D'Esposito, Michael P. Alexander
Neurology Jan 1995, 45 (1) 38-41; DOI: 10.1212/WNL.45.1.38

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Permissions

Make Comment

See Comments

Downloads
593

Share

  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Loading

Abstract

Article abstract-Numerous reports of aphasia after subcortical lesions have produced incomplete agreement about basic clinico-anatomic correlations. Some disagreement has arisen from methodologic differences. To control for some of the common differences, we analyzed 13 patients with left putaminal hemorrhage controlled for location--subcortical but not thalamic, and for time postonset--studied in both acute and postacute epochs. There was no apparent correlation between lesion site and acute language profiles. During the postacute epoch, there were several distinct correlations between lesion site (postacute decreased CT density) and specific aphasia dimensions--nonfluency, impaired comprehension, and perhaps impaired repetition. Our correlations were compatible with comparably controlled cases in the literature. A corollary result of this study is that patients fluent during the early epoch are likely to have a better outcome, and those initially nonfluent have a poor prognosis for language recovery.

NEUROLOGY 1995;45: 38-41

Aphasia due to subcortical vascular events occurs [1-14] after either ischemic infarction or hemorrhage of the striatocapsular region, [5,12] thalamus, [7,9] or paraventricular white matter (PVWM) [1,12]. These numerous studies have not reached a clear consensus regarding the specific correlations between lesion site and corresponding speech and language deficits, even in symposia designed to reach consensus [15]. Some investigators have even questioned if there are any clinico-anatomic correlations that can be drawn between the site of lesion and the pattern of language deficits in cases of subcortical aphasia [2,14].

Methodologic differences among studies may be the source of difficulty in demonstrating consistent clinico-anatomic relationships with subcortical lesions. Previous studies had mixed stroke etiologies, including both subcortical ischemic and hemorrhagic cases [1,2,4,14]. Early mass effect in the hemorrhage cases can lead to nonspecific disturbances in global attention that can obscure clinico-anatomic relationships during the acute epoch. Only postacute testing might be relevant, and it is not always available. Many studies have assessed patients (with infarctions or hemorrhage) a single time at widely variable times postonset of illness [13,14]. Because aphasia often improves with time, patients are being studied at potentially varying points in recovery. Some earlier studies also have mixed anatomic regions, including examples of both thalamic and nonthalamic lesions [2,14]. Thalamic damage probably causes aphasia through different neuropsychologic mechanisms [8] than damage to the striatocapsular region. Finally, differing assessment strategies for aphasia are utilized in different centers. Description of language deficits in a strictly taxonomic fashion [11] versus a loosely taxonomic fashion [3,4,14] could make comparisons among studies difficult.

Controlling for these methodologic factors should improve the specificity of lesion site relationships with aphasia profiles following subcortical vascular events. An initial study by our group [10] found a consistent "core" profile among patients with left capsulostriatal infarctions. We now report our analysis of aphasia after left putaminal hemorrhage. We had two hypotheses: (1) the acute language profile of these patients would have weak anatomic correlations; (2) the late language profile would consist of several distinct profiles that are each associated with specific lesion sites. A greater variability in lesion location and aphasia profile, as compared to ischemic infarcts, can occur because hemorrhages can produce lesions beyond usual vascular territories.

Methods. Subjects. The subjects of this study are thirteen consecutive patients with CT-demonstrated left putaminal hemorrhages from whom both early and late studies could be obtained who had been admitted to Braintree Rehabilitation Hospital, Braintree, MA. There were nine men and four women with a mean age of 60.0 (SD, 16.2). All patients were right-handed except for patient 4 Table 1. None of the patients had a history of previous neurologic events. Since patients were evaluated in a rehabilitation hospital, it is likely that there was a bias toward patients having sensorimotor impairments. All patients had severe hemiparesis (except nos. 1 and 2) and severe hemisensory deficits (except nos. 1 to 5).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint

Table 1. Speech and language data during the late assessment as well as the pattern of language recovery by aphasia taxonomic category

Clinical evaluation. Patients were evaluated twice. Early assessment was done 2 to 4 weeks following the acute illness. Late assessment was done 8 to 10 weeks postonset. Formal speech and language evaluation consisted of the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) [16]. Case-by-case profiles were assembled using speech quality, fluency, repetition, naming, type of paraphasia, and comprehension.

Neuroimaging. All patients had CTs during the acute phase that documented intracerebral hemorrhage (ICH) as the stroke etiology. None of the patients had lesions elsewhere in the brain. All follow-up CTs were performed more than 2 months postonset. Furthermore, all follow-up CTs that were used for lesion localization showed no evidence of residual blood. Lesion localization was determined with a template method previously described Figure 1 [1].

Figure1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Figure 1. Schematic drawings of CTs of patients with (A) "core" language deficit, (B) persistent impairment in repetition, (C) persistent nonfluency, (D) persistent impairment in comprehension, and (E) persistent impairment in repetition and comprehension

Results. Early assessment. All 13 patients were aphasic--six fluent and seven nonfluent. All seven nonfluent patients had impaired comprehension and repetition and fit the WAB criteria of global aphasia. Only two fluent patients had impaired comprehension, one with normal repetition (by WAB criteria--transcortical sensory aphasia) and one with impaired repetition (Wernicke's aphasia). The other four patients had fluent output, ie, sentence length and grammatic, but it was terse and often delayed. These four patients had only impaired naming on the WAB (anomic aphasia). Using the area of increased density (hemorrhage) on the acute scan as the lesion site, no relationship of lesion site could be distinguished between fluent (N = 6) or nonfluent (N = 7) output or between intact (N = 5) or impaired (N = 8) comprehension.

Late assessment. Speech/language testing during the late assessment is summarized in the table. Five of the six patients who were initially fluent (nos. 1 to 5) evolved to having a very mild aphasia with minimal or no anomia but poor verbal fluency, and some had mild initiation delay. All of these patients had late lesions within the capsulostriatal and paraventricular region Figure 1, A). The only left-handed patient was in this group and had a language profile that was entirely typical. The last fluent patient (no. 6) showed improvement in comprehension but had a persistent impairment in repetition with abundant phonemic paraphasias (conduction aphasia by WAB criteria). This patient had a lesion that extended laterally into the external capsule and insula and just into the temporal white matter Figure 1, B).

Three of the initially nonfluent patients (nos. 7 to 9) improved to having fluent output. All three had persistent impairments in repetition and comprehension (Wernicke's aphasia). The other four initially nonfluent patients (nos. 10 to 13) remained nonfluent. Three (nos. 10 to 12) had persistently impaired repetition but developed good comprehension (Broca's aphasia). The fourth (no. 13) had persistent impairments in repetition and comprehension (global aphasia). All patients with prolonged impaired comprehension had lesions that extended posteriorly from the capsulostriatal area into the temporal white matter area between the posterior limit of the sylvian fissure and the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle Figure 1, C). All patients with persistently impaired fluency had postacute low-density CT lesions that included the capsulostriatal region but extended anteriorly to involve much of the deep frontal white matter Figure 1, D). The patient with both persistently impaired fluency and comprehension had a lesion that extended both anteriorly and posteriorly Figure 1, E). The pattern of recovery based on WAB aphasia taxonomic categories is presented in the table. Sixty-two percent of these patients (8 of 13) changed aphasia profiles from the early to the late epoch.

Discussion. Many prior reports on "subcortical aphasia" have classified aphasia profiles by classic syndromes and have largely failed to find a unifying taxonomy. Small lesions often produce no deficits on standard aphasia tests, and large lesions, sharing only some element of capsulostriatal damage, varied quite dramatically in language deficits. Our companion study [10] of nonlacunar infarctions in lenticulostriatal distributions suggested that the core deficit in language largely spares word comprehension and responsive language--repetition, oral reading, and even in the mildest case, confrontational naming. In addition, syntax and grammar for short conversational responses are spared. The deficits in this core syndrome are in "generative" language [17,18]--verbal fluency, sentence generation, and discourse. Damage to frontal-striatal systems [1] responsible for complex proceduralized output underlies this deficit profile, whether damage occurs at the dorsolateral frontal cortex, [19] dorsolateral striatum, [20] or their connections in the deep frontal white matter [21].

Analysis of ICH adds a layer of clinical complexity because of possible acute nonspecific effects of hemorrhage mass and uncertainty about the precise site of actual damage. This problem may account for disparate conclusions reached in prior studies of aphasia after ICH [22] or in studies that mixed ICH with infarction in the acute stage [2,4,12,14]. In the present study we could identify no distinction in lesions of the basal ganglia, internal capsule, and deep frontal or deep temporal regions, between the acute ICH sites of patients with fluent versus nonfluent output or with preserved versus impaired word comprehension.

Restudied weeks later when blood had cleared on CT and edema had abated, the lesion-language relationships were clearer. Several patients with generally (but not uniformly) smaller lesions had become essentially nonaphasic by standard measures. All had residual lesions restricted to the putamen and middle PVWM. This is the same lesion-aphasia relationship described in the cases of ischemic infarction. Had we systematically probed residual generative language capacity--verbal fluency, [23] sentence generation, [17,18] or discourse [24]--we might have found mild reductions.

For the patients with persistent aphasia at postacute assessment, the lesion correlations with nonfluency, impaired word comprehension, and repetition were entirely compatible with theories of the distributed anatomy of language functions [25]. Nonfluency was most likely to occur with damage to a large portion of the deep frontal and PVWM. This finding is generally compatible with the theory of Naeser et al [26] regarding specific "pathways for fluency". There are numerous pathways through this region that may play a role in maintaining fluency but none is the nonfluency pathway. These could include the subcallosal fasciculus, [26] the projections from the supplementary motor area to dorsolateral frontal convexity that pass through the superior PVWM, [27] the lateral thalamic projections to Broca's area, the premotor callosal pathways, and even the projections from posterior association cortex passing caudorostrally in the PVWM [28]. Infarction can occur in this pattern involving the junctional area between the lenticulostriate and deep cortical perforator territories [29].

Impaired word comprehension was associated with damage into the deep temporal white matter, presumably damaging thalamotemporal projections [1] as well as short temporal association pathways within the temporal lobe. The mechanism of impaired comprehension is presumably similar to that seen after deep temporal ICH [30]. Although it may occur (case 13 in Alexander et al [1]), this is not a common pattern of damage often seen with infarction because it would require merging of the territories of the lenticulostriate and inferior cortical division of the middle cerebral artery, and even possibly, the anterior choroidal circulation [31].

Impaired repetition, despite preserved fluent output and comprehension, was present in one of our patients. Based on only a single patient, our explanation is tentative, but lesion extension into the subcortical parietal white matter may be the lesion correlate. The relevant pathways could include the arcuate fasciculus, extreme capsule, and short temporoparietal association connections. This specific subinsular/parietal lesion-aphasia correlation is seen with infarction, [32] but in patients with primarily subcortical infarctions, it is unusual. We are not certain that an example of lenticulostriate infarction with lateral extension has been reported.

While the literature reports are confounded by these issues of mixed vascular etiologies and variable times postonset, there are several cases that are suitable for fitting to our proposal about core lesions plus extensions after ICH. In 16 reports from four studies, [1,2,4,14] there is good agreement in 15 cases. Four (cases 1 and 6 in Puel et al [14] and 17 and 18 in Alexander et al [1]) had a speech/language profile consistent with the core subcortical aphasia profile [10] and appropriately had only putaminal lesions. One (case 2-3 in Cappa et al [4]) had a transcortical motor aphasia with a lesion in the caudate and anterior limb of the internal capsule, but this probably represents limited generative language deficits [10]. One (case 9 in Puel et al [14]) had a global aphasia and a lesion that extended anteriorly to involve the frontal isthmus and posteriorly to involve the temporal isthmus. Four (cases 3 and 4 in Puel et al [14] and 15 and 16 in Alexander et al [1]) had a profile of Wernicke's aphasia and had evidence of a lesion that extended posteriorly to involve the temporal isthmus. Five (cases 1-1 and 2-2 in Cappa et al, [4] 8 and 10 in Puel et al, [14] and 19 in Alexander et al [1]) had a profile of conduction aphasia with evidence of lateral extension to involve the extreme capsule and insula. Finally, there was one case that did not fit our model, but only acute data was published. This case (5 in Puel et al [14]) was reported to have Broca's aphasia, but there was no anterior extension of the lesion.

Weiller et al [33] recently proposed that aphasia in cases with subcortical infarction is actually due to the incidental microscopic cortical ischemic damage caused by the carotid or middle cerebral obstructive lesions that produce the large subcortical infarction. There are numerous problems with this proposal. The aphasia test used by these investigators does not easily identify transcortical aphasias or the mild generative aphasia of subcortical lesions. That a purely cortical lesion--even a macroscopic one--can produce standard Broca's or Wernicke's aphasia has never been demonstrated. We are also unaware of any reports of standard Broca's or Wernicke's aphasia after diffuse hypoxic-ischemic cortical damage. Furthermore, if aphasia profiles after nonthalamic subcortical hemorrhages are similar to the profiles produced by infarctions in the same territories, the cortical explanation of Weiller et al [33] seems unnecessary.

A corollary result of this study is information about the natural history of recovery of aphasia after lenticulostriate ICH. Patients fluent during the early epoch are likely to have a better outcome. The worst outcome in our initially fluent patients was a persistent impairment in repetition. On the other hand, none of the patients that were nonfluent during the early epoch had a very good outcome. These patients all had persistent impairments in fluency, comprehension, or both.

  • Copyright 1995 by Modern Medicine Publications, Inc., a subsidiary of Edgell Communications, Inc.

REFERENCES

  1. 1.↵
    Alexander MP, Naeser MA, Palumbo CL. Correlations of subcortical CT lesion sites and aphasia profiles. Brain 1987;110:961-991.
    OpenUrl
  2. 2.↵
    Basso A, Della Sala S, Farabola M. Aphasia arising from purely deep lesions. Cortex 1987;23:29-44.
    OpenUrl
  3. 3.↵
    Brunner RJ, Kornhuber HH, Seemuller E, Suger G, Wallesch C-W. Basal ganglia participation in language pathology. Brain Lang 1982;16:281-299.
    OpenUrl
  4. 4.↵
    Cappa SF, Cavallotti G, Guidotti M, et al. Subcortical aphasia: two clinical-CT scan correlation studies. Cortex 1983;19:227-241.
    OpenUrl
  5. 5.↵
    Damasio AR, Damasio H, Rizzo M, et al. Aphasia with nonhemorrhagic lesions in the basal ganglia and internal capsule. Arch Neurol 1982;39:15-20.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. 6.
    Demonet JF, Puel M, Celsis P, Cardebat D. "Subcortical" aphasia: some proposed pathophysiological mechanisms and their rCBF correlates revealed by SPECT. J Neurolinguistics 1991;6:319-344.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  7. 7.↵
    Gorelick PB, Hier DB, Benevento L, et al. Aphasia after left thalamic infarction. Arch Neurol 1984;41:1296-1298.
    OpenUrl
  8. 8.↵
    Graff-Radford NR, Damasio H, Yamada T, et al. Nonhemorrhagic thalamic infarction: clinical, neuropsychological, and electrophysiological findings in four anatomic groups defined by computed tomography. Brain 1985;108:485-516.
    OpenUrl
  9. 9.
    McFarling D, Rothi LJ, Heilman KM. Transcortical aphasia from ischemic infarcts of the thalamus: a report of two cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1982;45:107-112.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. 10.↵
    Mega MS, Alexander MP. Subcortical aphasia: the core profile of capsulostriatal infarction. Neurology 1994;44:1824-1829.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. 11.↵
    Metter EJ, Riege WH, Hanson WR, et al. Subcortical structures in aphasia: an analysis based on (F-18)-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography and computed tomography. Arch Neurol 1988;45:1229-1235.
    OpenUrl
  12. 12.
    Naeser MA, Alexander MP, Helm-Estabrooks N, et al. Aphasia with predominantly subcortical lesion sites: description of three capsular/putaminal aphasia syndromes. Arch Neurol 1982;39:2-14.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. 13.↵
    Perani D, Vallar G, Cappa S, et al. Aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke: a clinical/cerebral perfusion correlation study. Brain 1987;110:1211-1229.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. 14.↵
    Puel M, Demonet JF, Cardebat D, et al. Aphasies sous-corticales: etude neurolinguistique avec scanner X de 25 cas. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1984;140:695-710.
    OpenUrl
  15. 15.↵
    Vallar G, Cappa S, Wallesch CW. Neuropsychological disorders associated with subcortical lesions. New York: Oxford University Press, 1992.
  16. 16.↵
    Kertesz A. Western aphasia battery. London, ON, Canada: University of Western Ontario, 1980.
  17. 17.↵
    Luria A, Tsvetkova L. Towards the mechanism of "dynamic aphasia". Acta Neurol Psychiat Belg 1967;67:1045-1057.
    OpenUrl
  18. 18.
    Rubens AR. Transcortical motor aphasia. In: Whitaker H, Whitaker HA, eds. Studies in neurolinguistics, vol 1. New York: Academic Press, 1976:293-306.
  19. 19.↵
    Alexander MP, Benson DF, Stuss DT. Frontal lobes and language. Brain Lang 1989;37:656-691.
    OpenUrl
  20. 20.↵
    Mendez MF, Adams NL, Lewandowski KS. Neurobehavioral changes associated with caudate lesions. Neurology 1989;39:349-354.
    OpenUrl
  21. 21.↵
    Alexander MP. Speech and language deficits after subcortical lesions of the left hemisphere; a clinical, CT, and PET study. In: Vallar G, Cappa SF, Wallesch CW, eds. Neuropsychological disorders associated with subcortical lesions. New York: Oxford University Press, 1992:455-477.
  22. 22.↵
    Alexander MP, LoVerme SR Jr. Aphasia after left hemispheric intracerebral hemorrhage. Neurology 1980;30:1193-1202.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  23. 23.↵
    Milner B. Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In: Warren J, Akert K, eds. The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1964:313-334.
  24. 24.↵
    Nadeau SE. Impaired grammar with normal fluency and phonology: implications for Broca's aphasia. Brain 1988;111:1111-1137.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. 25.↵
    Mesulam M-M. Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing in attention, language and memory. Ann Neurol 1990;28:597-613.
    OpenUrl
  26. 26.↵
    Naeser MA, Palumbo CL, Helm-Estabrooks N, et al. Severe nonfluency in aphasia: role of the medial subcallosal fasciculus and other white matter pathways in recovery of spontaneous speech. Brain 1989;112:1-38.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. 27.↵
    Jurgens U. The efferent and afferent connections of the supplementary motor area. Brain Res 1984;300:63-81.
    OpenUrl
  28. 28.↵
    Nauta WJH, Feirtag M. Fundamental neuroanatomy. New York: WH Freeman & Co, 1986.
  29. 29.↵
    Bogousslavsky J, Regli F. Centrum ovale infarcts: subcortical infarction in the superficial territory of the middle cerebral artery. Neurology 1992;42:1992-1998.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  30. 30.↵
    Kase CS, Williams JP, Wyatt DA, Mohr JP. Lobar intracerebral hematomas: clinical and CT analysis of 22 cases. Neurology 1982;32:1146-1150.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  31. 31.↵
    Percheron G. Les arteres du thalamus humain: territoire des arteres choroidiennes. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1977;133:547-559.
    OpenUrl
  32. 32.↵
    Damasio H, Damasio AR. The anatomical basis of conduction aphasia. Brain 1980;103:337-350.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  33. 33.↵
    Weiller C, Willmes K, Reiche W, et al. The case of aphasia or neglect after striatocapsular infarction. Brain 1994; 116:1509-1525.

Disputes & Debates: Rapid online correspondence

No comments have been published for this article.
Comment

NOTE: All authors' disclosures must be entered and current in our database before comments can be posted. Enter and update disclosures at http://submit.neurology.org. Exception: replies to comments concerning an article you originally authored do not require updated disclosures.

  • Stay timely. Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
  • Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
  • 200 words maximum.
  • 5 references maximum. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
  • 5 authors maximum. Exception: replies can include all original authors of the article.
  • Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.

More guidelines and information on Disputes & Debates

Compose Comment

More information about text formats

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
Author Information
NOTE: The first author must also be the corresponding author of the comment.
First or given name, e.g. 'Peter'.
Your last, or family, name, e.g. 'MacMoody'.
Your email address, e.g. higgs-boson@gmail.com
Your role and/or occupation, e.g. 'Orthopedic Surgeon'.
Your organization or institution (if applicable), e.g. 'Royal Free Hospital'.
Publishing Agreement
NOTE: All authors, besides the first/corresponding author, must complete a separate Disputes & Debates Submission Form and provide via email to the editorial office before comments can be posted.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.

Vertical Tabs

You May Also be Interested in

Back to top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Advertisement

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.

Alert Me

  • Alert me when eletters are published
Neurology: 96 (15)

Articles

  • Ahead of Print
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Popular Articles
  • Translations

About

  • About the Journals
  • Ethics Policies
  • Editors & Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertise

Submit

  • Author Center
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Information for Reviewers
  • AAN Guidelines
  • Permissions

Subscribers

  • Subscribe
  • Activate a Subscription
  • Sign up for eAlerts
  • RSS Feed
Site Logo
  • Visit neurology Template on Facebook
  • Follow neurology Template on Twitter
  • Visit Neurology on YouTube
  • Neurology
  • Neurology: Clinical Practice
  • Neurology: Genetics
  • Neurology: Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • AAN.com
  • AANnews
  • Continuum
  • Brain & Life
  • Neurology Today

Wolters Kluwer Logo

Neurology | Print ISSN:0028-3878
Online ISSN:1526-632X

© 2021 American Academy of Neurology

  • Privacy Policy
  • Feedback
  • Advertise