Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Inclusion, Diversity, Equity, Anti-racism, & Social Justice (IDEAS)
    • Innovations in Care Delivery
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Author Center

Advanced Search

Main menu

  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Inclusion, Diversity, Equity, Anti-racism, & Social Justice (IDEAS)
    • Innovations in Care Delivery
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Author Center
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Neurology
Home
The most widely read and highly cited peer-reviewed neurology journal
  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
Site Logo
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

Share

February 01, 1999; 52 (3) Brief Communications

Education and the incidence of dementia in a large population-based study: The Rotterdam Study

A. Ott, C.T. M. van Rossum, F. van Harskamp, H. van de Mheen, A. Hofman, M.M. B. Breteler
First published February 1, 1999, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.52.3.663
A. Ott
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
C.T. M. van Rossum
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
F. van Harskamp
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
H. van de Mheen
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
A. Hofman
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
M.M. B. Breteler
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Citation
Education and the incidence of dementia in a large population-based study: The Rotterdam Study
A. Ott, C.T. M. van Rossum, F. van Harskamp, H. van de Mheen, A. Hofman, M.M. B. Breteler
Neurology Feb 1999, 52 (3) 663; DOI: 10.1212/WNL.52.3.663

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Permissions

Make Comment

See Comments

Downloads
3

Share

  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Loading

Abstract

Article abstract We assessed the risk of dementia by educational level in a prospective population-based study. In the Rotterdam Study, 6,827 nondemented participants with known education level were followed for an average of 2.1 years. During this period, 137 new cases of dementia occurred. Low education was associated with higher dementia risk in women but not in men, suggesting that the association is modified by sex. Our data indicate that cross-sectional studies may overrate the association between education and risk of dementia.

Many studies have examined the relation between dementia and educational background.1 Studies reporting an inverse association between education and dementia have been criticized for possible methodologic flaws, such as selection or diagnostic bias.2 Meanwhile, hypotheses were formulated to explain the relation: higher brain reserve capacity of educated persons could postpone the onset of dementia3; educated elderly may get more brain stimulation (“use it or lose it”)3,4; and factors related to education, such as lifestyle, occupational exposure, morbidity, and health care, may be responsible for the association with dementia.2,4 Findings from the Nun Study suggested that poor cognitive function and AD were associated with limited linguistic ability at younger age.5

Most studies reporting an inverse relation between education and dementia have been cross-sectional.1 We previously reported a strong cross-sectional association between low education and presence of dementia in a large population-based study.6 We now have information on dementia incidence in the same population that enables a more reliable prospective study of the relation.

Methods.

Study design.

The Rotterdam Study, a community-based cohort study among persons 55 years and older, was approved by the Erasmus University Medical Ethics Committee.7 The study focused on disabling diseases in the elderly and was conducted in a suburb of Rotterdam. At baseline, 7,983 persons were included in the study (response rate 78%). From all participants, we obtained written informed consent and permission to seek information from treating physicians. Baseline examinations took place from 1990 to 1993. At baseline, 6,827 study participants with data on educational level were nondemented and thus at risk for dementia.

After 2 to 3 years, participants were re-examined, using procedures similar to those used at baseline.6,8 Briefly, participants were screened with a short test of cognition (Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE] and the Geriatric Mental State schedule, organic level [GMS-A]). Those scoring below 26 points on the MMSE or more than 0 on the GMS-A were examined with the Cambridge examination for mental disorders of the elderly (CAMDEX) diagnostic interview, and when suspected of having dementia, referred for further diagnostic work-up. This included informant interview, neuropsychologic testing, examination by a neurologist, and, if possible, neuroimaging. Of the at-risk population, 510 persons (7.5%) died before follow-up and 845 (12.4%) were not re-examined in person, mainly due to refusal. To complete follow-up, we obtained information on those who were not re-examined from general practitioners and the regional institute for outpatient mental health care (RIAGG), which covers the entire study population. In the Netherlands, the RIAGG is responsible for dementia care facility indications. Examinations by the RIAGG include (informant) interviews, neurologic and neuropsychologic testing, blood biochemistry, and syphilis serology. Incidence rates appeared similar between those who were and those who were not re-examined in person. A panel that reviewed all available information made diagnoses according to current clinical criteria (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd ed., revised; National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association; and National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke–Association Internationale pour la Recherche et l’Enseignement en Neurosciences).

At baseline, attained level of education was assessed by interview and grouped according to the Standard Classification of Education (SOI), which is comparable with the International Standard Classification of Education (UNESCO, Paris, 1976). In the SOI, seven levels are recognized, ranging from primary education to university level. We combined the four highest levels into one category, high-level education, equivalent to at least 11 years of education. The second and third levels (low-level vocational training and medium-level secondary education) were combined to make a category of medium-level education, equivalent to 7 to 10 years of education. The lowest level contained persons with primary school only (low level education, less than 7 years of education). This category includes persons who did not complete primary school or those without any formal education (<1%).

Data analysis.

We calculated education- and sex-specific incidence per 10-year age band. Relative risk of dementia and subtypes of dementia was assessed with a proportional hazards model, including both linear and squared age. We used the highest level of education as reference. Interaction between gender and education was tested by including a product term in a model containing gender and education. Because this interaction was statistically significant we calculated relative risks for men and women separately. To compare the effect of education between men and women, we calculated relative risks for all gender and level-of-education combinations, using highly educated women as the reference.

Results.

Table 1 shows baseline characteristics of the study population. The average age at baseline of men was slightly lower than that of women (68.2 versus 70.0); however, in the highest age category, which is at highest risk of dementia, women were over-represented. In this elderly population, men received more schooling than women: 50.6% of men versus 24.3% of women had a high level of education, whereas 26.4% of men versus 46.1% of women were low-educated.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of the study population

Duration of follow-up was on average 2.1 years, ranging from a few days for someone who died to 6.1 years (first quartile 1.4, median 1.9, third quartile 2.6 years). During follow-up, we diagnosed incident dementia in 137 persons. Of those, 97 had AD (71%) and 18 had vascular dementia (13%). Most patients had mild dementia; only one fifth had a moderately severe dementia. Age-specific incidence rates of dementia did not differ significantly between men and women.

Table 2 gives results on the incidence of total dementia by level of education. Among women, those with lowest education had the highest and those with highest education the lowest incidence of dementia in all age categories. For men there was no clear pattern.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Incidence of dementia by age, sex, and education level

We found a significant interaction between gender and education (p = 0.023). The relation between education and the risk of dementia was significant in women (table 3), but not in men. Comparing all gender and level-of-education combinations showed that the reference group of highly educated women had the lowest relative risk of dementia. Relative risks of low, medium, and highly educated men were 1.2 (95% CI 0.5 to 2.8), 1.7 (0.8 to 4.1), and 1.8 (0.9 to 3.7), and did not significantly differ from the reference group.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Relative risk (95% CI) of incident dementia by education level

Results for AD did not differ from those for total dementia (table 3). Because of limited power, we did not analyze the association with other subtypes of dementia.

Discussion.

This prospective study has several advantages over others that examined the association between education and dementia. The study is population-based with a high response rate. We also obtained complete follow-up of all participants at risk. Therefore, there is little room for selection bias. As opposed to register-based studies, we actively screened for dementia, which prevented bias by differences in health care utilization. The study was prospective and we assessed educational level before onset of dementia, thereby minimizing recall bias. In cross-sectional studies, persons with lifelong low cognitive abilities may erroneously be diagnosed as demented.1,4 This diagnostic bias is unlikely in the current study of incident dementia. In addition, we used similar screening and diagnostic procedures at baseline and follow-up. One possible diagnostic bias in our and other studies may have occurred because screening tests are more sensitive in picking up cognitive deficits associated with early dementia in lower than in more highly educated subjects; this could result in an underestimate of the risk of dementia in more highly educated persons.

We previously described the association between education and prevalent dementia.6 Similar to most other cross-sectional studies,1 our previous study suggested a stronger association than did our current results (age- and sex-adjusted odds ratio of low education and dementia 2.7, 95% CI 1.9 to 4.0). In that study, however, we could not avoid all the abovementioned biases. Owing to selection, disproportionately few higher-educated dementia patients may have been included. Stern et al. found that highly educated patients survived shorter than lower educated ones,9 and hypothesized that dementia in the higher educated remains unrecognized until a more progressed stage. However, a later study could not replicate these findings.10 A prevalence study of dementia is based on existing cases of dementia from which the proportion of dementia in a population is calculated. Prevalent dementia case series consist of dementia patients diagnosed in a prevalence study. It may be that high educated dementia patients are less willing than are low educated patients to participate in a prevalence study. Still, their risk of dementia may be similar. Our results may illustrate that cross-sectional studies overrate the relation of education with dementia risk.

The current results suggest that sex modifies the association between education and dementia. However, relative risk estimates in men were unstable, as reflected in wide confidence intervals. In line with the age distribution of the general population, there were more women than men in our study, in particular in the oldest age category. It is unlikely, however, that this explains the gender differences, because the difference seemed present in all age categories (see table 2). In regression analyses we thoroughly adjusted for age. A difference in the association by sex has not been described before. Reanalysis of our cross-sectional data also revealed a difference: the relation between low education and dementia was much stronger in women than in men (odds ratio 4.7 versus 1.8), which supports our current observation. In our study population, as a result of cohort effects, women were on average less well educated. One could speculate that in this age group educational level represents a different quality for men than for women. Education could, in women more than in men, reflect socioeconomic status rather than intellectual capacity. Alternatively, it is conceivable that the interaction between sex and education in the risk of dementia resulted from a sex difference in education-related dementia prognosis or risk factor profile.

Acknowledgments

Supported by the NESTOR stimulation program for geriatric research in the Netherlands (Ministry of Health and Ministry of Education), the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), the Netherlands Prevention Fund, and the municipality of Rotterdam.

Acknowledgment

The authors are indebted to the general practitioners in Ommoord and the RIAGG Rijnmond Noord-Oost.

  • Received May 27, 1997.
  • Accepted October 24, 1998.

References

  1. ↵
    Katzman R. Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1993;43:13–20.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    Beard CM, Kokmen E, Offord KP, Kurland LT. Lack of association between Alzheimer’s disease and education, occupation, marital status, or living arrangement. Neurology 1992;42:2063–2068.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Stern Y, Alexander GE, Prohovnik I, et al. Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow : implications for a reserve against Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Neurology 1995;45:55–60.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    Albert MS. How does education affect cognitive function? Ann Epidemiol 1995;5:76–78.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Snowdon DA, Kemper SJ, Mortimer JA, Greiner LH, Wekstein DR, Markesbery WR. Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life. Findings from the Nun Study. JAMA 1996;275:528–532.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Ott A, Breteler MMB, van Harskamp F, et al. Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia : association with education. The Rotterdam study. BMJ 1995;310:970–973.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Hofman A, Grobbee DE, de Jong PTVM, van den Ouweland FA. Determinants of disease and disability in the elderly. The Rotterdam Elderly Study. Eur J Epidemiol 1991;7:403–422.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Ott A, Breteler MMB, van Harskamp F, Stijnen T, Hofman A. Incidence and risk of dementia. The Rotterdam Study. Am J Epidemiol 1998;147:574–580.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Stern Y, Tang MX, Denaro J, Mayeux R. Increased risk of mortality in Alzheimer’s disease patients with more advanced educational and occupational attainment. Ann Neurol 1995;37:590–595.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Geerlings MI, Deeg DJ, Schmand B, Lindeboom J, Jonker C. Increased risk of mortality in Alzheimer’s disease patients with higher education? A replication study. Neurology 1997;49:798–802.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text

Letters: Rapid online correspondence

No comments have been published for this article.
Comment

REQUIREMENTS

If you are uploading a letter concerning an article:
You must have updated your disclosures within six months: http://submit.neurology.org

Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.

If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.

Submission specifications:

  • Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
  • Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
  • Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
  • Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
  • Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.

More guidelines and information on Disputes & Debates

Compose Comment

More information about text formats

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
Author Information
NOTE: The first author must also be the corresponding author of the comment.
First or given name, e.g. 'Peter'.
Your last, or family, name, e.g. 'MacMoody'.
Your email address, e.g. higgs-boson@gmail.com
Your role and/or occupation, e.g. 'Orthopedic Surgeon'.
Your organization or institution (if applicable), e.g. 'Royal Free Hospital'.
Publishing Agreement
NOTE: All authors, besides the first/corresponding author, must complete a separate Publishing Agreement Form and provide via email to the editorial office before comments can be posted.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.

Vertical Tabs

You May Also be Interested in

Back to top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Methods.
    • Results.
    • Discussion.
    • Acknowledgments
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Advertisement

Hemiplegic Migraine Associated With PRRT2 Variations A Clinical and Genetic Study

Dr. Robert Shapiro and Dr. Amynah Pradhan

► Watch

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.

Alert Me

  • Alert me when eletters are published
Neurology: 100 (5)

Articles

  • Ahead of Print
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Popular Articles
  • Translations

About

  • About the Journals
  • Ethics Policies
  • Editors & Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertise

Submit

  • Author Center
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Information for Reviewers
  • AAN Guidelines
  • Permissions

Subscribers

  • Subscribe
  • Activate a Subscription
  • Sign up for eAlerts
  • RSS Feed
Site Logo
  • Visit neurology Template on Facebook
  • Follow neurology Template on Twitter
  • Visit Neurology on YouTube
  • Neurology
  • Neurology: Clinical Practice
  • Neurology: Education
  • Neurology: Genetics
  • Neurology: Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • AAN.com
  • AANnews
  • Continuum
  • Brain & Life
  • Neurology Today

Wolters Kluwer Logo

Neurology | Print ISSN:0028-3878
Online ISSN:1526-632X

© 2023 American Academy of Neurology

  • Privacy Policy
  • Feedback
  • Advertise