Neuropsychological changes between “off” and “on” STN or GPi stimulation in Parkinson’s disease
Citation Manager Formats
Make Comment
See Comments

Abstract
Background: In a previous study on a consecutive series of 62 patients with PD, the authors showed that bilateral subthalamic or pallidal continuous high-frequency deep brain stimulation (DBS) affects neither memory nor executive functions 3 to 6 months after surgery.
Objective: To investigate the specific effects of DBS by comparing the performance of patients with the stimulator turned “on” and “off.”
Methods: The performance of 56 patients on clinical tests of executive function was compared after 3 and 12 months of DBS of the subthalamic nucleus (STN; n = 48) or the internal globus pallidus (GPi; n = 8) with the stimulator “on” or “off.” Global intellectual efficiency, verbal learning, and mood were also evaluated with the stimulator “on.” The performance of another group of 20 patients was compared after 6 months of DBS of the STN (n = 15) or the GPi (n = 5) with the stimulator “on” or “off” on more experimental tests recently shown to be more sensitive to l-dopa therapy.
Results: When the stimulator was “on,” STN patients showed a mild but significant improvement in psychomotor speed and working memory. In comparison with the presurgical state, STN patients had no cognitive deficit at 12 months, except for lexical fluency. There was no differential effect of STN or GPi stimulation.
Conclusions: 1) The specific effect of DBS seems to mimic the action of l-dopa treatment in the cognitive as in the motor domain; 2) the surgery associated with DBS does not appear to affect the cognitive performance of patients with PD 12 months later, except for a mild deficit in lexical fluency.
The failure of levodopa and dopaminergic medication to achieve long-term symptom relief in patients with PD, coupled with an improvement in our knowledge of basal ganglia pathophysiology and advances in stereotactic techniques, have led to renewed interest in surgical treatments, through pallidotomy and continuous high-frequency deep brain stimulation (DBS). Posteroventral pallidotomy improves the cardinal symptoms of PD and levodopa-induced dyskinesias, particularly on the side contralateral to the surgical lesion,1,2 but consistent cognitive deficits may be demonstrated when the group of patients is sufficiently large.3,4 DBS, a reversible nonlesioning surgical treatment,5 can be applied to the internal globus pallidus (GPi)6 or subthalamic nucleus (STN)7 with a significant bilateral reduction in parkinsonian disability.8,9 Compared to before surgery, there was no significant change in memory or executive functions 3 to 6 months after DBS in a series of 62 patients with PD treated by bilateral STN or GPi stimulation.10
These results are, however, open to discussion.11 For instance, surgery could have provoked cognitive
deficits compensated for by stimulation. The primaryaim of this new study was thus to distinguish the effects of DBS from those of surgery by comparing the postsurgical performance of patients with the stimulator turned “on” and “off.” In the motor domain, the effects of DBS are reported to mimic those of l-dopa therapy.12 To ascertain whether this is also the case in the cognitive domain, we compared the neuropsychological performance of patients with the stimulator turned “on” and “off,” first on classic cognitive tests sensitive to PD13 and then on more experimental tasks recently shown to be more specifically sensitive to the action of l-dopa.14,15 Alternatively, cognitive deficits could appear later as a result of repetitive and prolonged stimulation. Thus, the second aim of the study was to assess cognitive function over a 12-month follow-up.
Methods.
Patients.
The study included 76 patients with PD (table 1). A first group of 56 patients underwent bilateral implantation of electrodes in Grenoble, 48 in the STN (STN1 group) and 8 in the GPi (GPi1 group). A second group of 20 patients underwent bilateral implantation of electrodes in Paris, 15 in the STN (STN2 group) and 5 in the GPi (GPi2 group). All had a severe form of the disease and the response to levodopa was clear in all patients as shown by the Unified PD Rating Scale scores.16 Despite optimal medication based on a combination of levodopa and dopamine receptor agonists, severe motor fluctuations were observed in all patients. No other neurologic impairment was found and brain MRI was normal. Patients were relatively young and had no significant cognitive or mood impairment before surgery. The four groups of patients did not differ in terms of age, level of education, gender, or disease duration or severity.
Characteristics of the four groups of patients*
The neurosurgical procedures, all of which were approved by the relevant French Ethics Committee, have already been described.7,17 For all patients, the electrodes were implanted stereotactically in a single session in accordance with preoperative MRI and ventriculography and intraoperative microrecordings and stimulations. The implanted quadripolar electrodes were positioned as closely as possible to the location where motor benefit was induced by the lowest electrical intensity and adverse effects by the highest electrical intensity using monopolar stimulation.
A brain MRI was performed a few days after electrode implantation to check the final location of the electrodes and detect any possible surgical complications. Seven patients had intracranial bleeding or edema, but had recovered without sequelae by the time of the neuropsychological examination. All patients had some degree of mental slowness or confusion from a few hours to days, except two patients who recovered after 1 month. Postoperative MRI showed mild extracerebral bleeding in three patients, small intraventricular bleeding in two patients, and frontal area contusion along one electrode tract in two other patients. No other serious adverse events associated with electrode implantation were detected.
Electrical parameters (pulse width, frequency, and voltage) were progressively adjusted by telemetry, using a console programmer, until an optimal effect was reached, in both the “on” and “off” drug conditions. In all patients stimulation was monopolar, using one contact of the quadripolar electrode. Parkinsonian motor features improved in all patients. The improvement was more pronounced with STN stimulation and the antiparkinsonian medication could be decreased in these patients (see table 1). At the time of the study, the mean (SD) voltage of stimulation was 2.4 (0.7) V for the STN and 3.1 (0.6) V for the GPi; the mean pulse width was 60.5 (10.9) μs for the STN and 78.5 (28.8) μs for the GPi; the mean frequency was 137.0 (27.6) Hz for the STN and 139.6 (20.6) Hz for the GPi.
Neuropsychological assessment.
Clinical tests of executive function that usually indicate impairment in PD13 were performed in 56 patients (groups STN1 and GPi1) before (during the preceding month) and 3 and 12 months after electrode implantation. After surgery, the performance was also compared with the stimulator turned “on” and “off,” in a counterbalanced order. The conditions of stimulation were set from 15 to 30 minutes before beginning the neuropsychological examination. Most of the patients were assessed without levodopa (after 12 hours’ withdrawal): 51 of 56 at 3 months and 56 of 56 at 12 months. The assessment included the simplified version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test,18 verbal fluency tests,19 and graphic and motor series.20 To limit test–retest effects, parallel forms were used; for instance, for category fluency the names of fruit or furniture in 1 minute, and for literal fluency words beginning with “V” or “R” for 1 minute each. For the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test a parallel form was established with background, position, and form as criteria. The performance in both forms of the test was found to be similar in two groups of 40 normal subjects matched for age and educational level (form 1: 5.2 [1.2] criteria; form 2: 5.2 [1.2] criteria). The order of presentation of the parallel forms was counterbalanced. Given the potential role of control of attention on task performance, we added the Stroop Test,21 which estimates the inhibition of interference, and the Trail Making Test,22 which evaluates set-shifting.
Because clinical tests may lack sensitivity to subtle cognitive changes, we used more experimental tests of executive functions previously shown to be sensitive to l-dopa therapy. These tests were assessed in 20 patients (groups STN2 and GPi2) 6 months after electrode implantation and performance was compared with the stimulator turned “on” and with it turned “off” in a counterbalanced order. Given the patients’ difficulty in carrying out the tasks when in the l-dopa “off” and stimulation “off” condition, their l-dopa dose (see table 1) was maintained in both the “off” and “on” stimulation conditions. We selected tests from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB): Motor Screening and Big Little Circle for simple and choice reaction times, Intra and Extradimensional Set Shifting for cognitive flexibility, and Spatial Working Memory.14 We added the Digit Ordering Test for verbal working memory.15
Global intellectual efficiency, verbal learning, and mood were also evaluated in 56 patients (groups STN1 and GPi1) before (during the preceding month) and 3 and 12 months after electrode implantation (with the stimulators “on”). The Mattis Dementia Rating Scale allowed attention, initiation, construction, conceptualization, and memory to be assessed.23 To investigate verbal learning, we used the Grober and Buschke Test, which allows a comparison of free and cued recall.24,25 The 16 to-be-learned words, belonging to 16 different semantic categories, were presented to the subject on four different cards. The encoding phase was controlled by asking the subject to point to and read aloud each word when its category cue was verbally provided. Then, the card was removed and immediate cued recall was tested by providing each category cue. Whenever the subject was unable to recall a given item, the encoding procedure was performed again for this item until the correct answer was obtained. The recall phase for the 16 words included three trials. Each trial consisted of an extended period of free recall (up to 2 minutes), immediately followed by cued recall for those items not retrieved at free recall. Selective reminding was used for any item missed at cued recall. The verbal learning free recall score (from 0 to 48) was determined by the total number of words correctly evoked in the three trials. The verbal learning total recall score (from 0 to 48) was determined by the number of words correctly evoked at free or cued recall. Delayed verbal free recall and delayed verbal total recall scores were also measured after a 15-minute delay. Two parallel forms were used to control for retest effects. Mood was assessed by the Beck Depression Inventory.26
Data analysis.
Given the possible influence of locus of implantation (STN versus GPi), analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures was performed on each of the variables for each group separately, using condition (stimulator “off” and “on”) and assessment time (before surgery, 3 months and 12 months after surgery) as repeated measures. For variables that showed significant differences, the pattern of differences was examined by two-tailed t-tests. Nonparametric analysis (Freedman and Wilcoxon tests) was effected when required by small groups of patients or variance heterogeneity. Two-way ANOVA was also performed on each of the variables, using group as a between factor, condition (stimulator “off” and “on”) and assessment time (before surgery, 3 months and 12 months after surgery) as repeated measures.
Results.
For the STN group, there was a significant improvement under stimulation (stimulator “on” versus stimulator “off”) in: 1) graphic series, word and color conditions of the Stroop Test, and part A and B of the Trail Making Test, notably at 12 months (table 2); and 3 psychomotor latency in simple and choice reaction times (table 3); the number of errors in spatial working memory, particularly for the more complex level (see table 3). There was also a trend toward significance for verbal working memory (see table 3). These positive changes were less than one SD for most of the patients. There was a significant effect of time of assessment (before surgery, 3 months and 12 months after surgery) on: 1) category fluency, with a poorer performance after surgery with or without stimulation (table 2;) part B of the Trail Making Test, with a better performance at 12 months under stimulation (see table 2 and 3⇓) verbal free recall, with a lower performance at 3 months, but recovery at 12 months (table 4); mood, with a lower depression score after surgery (see table 4).
Comparison of performance on tests of executive function before electrode implantation and after, with stimulator turned “off” and “on”
Psychomotor speed, cognitive flexibility, verbal and spatial working memory with the stimulator turned “off” and “on”
Comparison of performance on tests of global cognitive efficiency, memory, and mood before and after electrode implantation (stimulator turned “on”)
For the GPi groups there was no significant improvement under stimulation (stimulator “on” versus stimulator “off”). There was a significant effect of time of assessment (before surgery, 3 months and 12 months after surgery) only on the Initiation subtest of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale with an improvement at 12 months (see table 4).
There was only one significant group effect (STN versus GPi; p < 0.05) with a longer response latency in simple choice reaction time for the GPi2 group (see table 3). There was only one significant interaction (p < 0.05) between group and condition of stimulation: the number of errors in the more complex level of spatial working memory decreased under stimulation in the STN2 group, whereas it increased in the GPi2 group (see table 3). There was also only one significant interaction between group and time of assessment: the score of the subtest of Initiation of the Mattis Dementia Scale increased at 12 months in the GPi1 group but not in the STN1 group (see table 4).
Discussion.
Our results showed cognitive improvement in psychomotor speed and working memory in STN patients when the stimulator was turned “on,” no overall differential effect between STN and GPi stimulation, and no cognitive long-term effect of DBS 12 months after surgery except for a mild lexical fluency deficit in STN patients.
On clinical tests of executive functions, the performance of STN patients improved with the stimulator turned “on” for the word and color condition of the Stroop test and the forms A and B of the Trail Making Test. This was mainly related to an increase in psychomotor speed, as there was no significant change in cognitive speed under stimulation for the interference condition of the Stroop test and the difference in time of execution between condition B (cognitive shifting) and condition A (simple tracking) of the Trail Making Test just failed significance. This increase in psychomotor speed was also found in simple (Motor Screening) and choice (Big Little Circle) reaction time tests from the CANTAB. The effects of STN stimulation were comparable to those found in patients treated with l-dopa or dopamine agonists in similar tasks.27 The improved performance on psychomotor tasks was congruent with the significant improvement of upper limb akinesia observed with the stimulator “on”7,9 and associated with a slight improvement of initiation time and a more marked improvement of movement execution time.12
More unexpected was the effect of stimulation on purely cognitive functions, given the relative independence of motor and cognitive subcorticofrontal circuits.28 This effect was significant in spatial working memory and showed a trend toward significance in verbal working memory. In spatial working memory, subjects were required to collect blue tokens randomly hidden inside boxes and not to reopen a box in which a blue token has already been found. This test was impaired by l-dopa withdrawal in levodopa-treated parkinsonian patients with more than 9 years of disease duration.14 In verbal working memory, subjects were read random series of seven digits and were required to reorder the items in memory and to repeat them in ascending fashion. This Digit Ordering Test was specifically improved from the untreated to the levodopa-treated state in de novo parkinsonian patients.15 Therefore, in the cognitive as in the motor domain, DBS appears to mimic the effects of levodopa therapy. This similarity is mainly phenomenologic, because levodopa acts by restoring dopamine neurotransmission in the striatum, whereas DBS is supposed to inhibit the STN or GPi. It turns out that close effects can be obtained through different neurophysiologic mechanisms, which are, however, functionally linked, as the striatal dopaminergic activation leads to a decrease in the subthalamic–pallidal pathway activity. This was shown both in animal models of PD and patients with PD.29,30 The degree of improvement of working memory was lower with DBS in our study than with levodopa therapy in the previous studies. However, a dose of l-dopa (see table 1) had to be maintained for these experimental tasks both at baseline (stimulator “off”) and under stimulation, due to the difficulty patients experienced in performing these tasks with stimulation “off” and levodopa “off.” This dose of levodopa may have improved the baseline condition. These working memory tasks require processes of self-ordering and monitoring that depend on the integrity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.31 A same or adjacent area, as far as the resolution of PET imaging can show, was significantly activated when STN stimulation was applied during random joystick movement.32
Why were the cognitive effects of DBS limited to tests of working memory? Patients with PD are known to be impaired in many tests of executive function.13 It should be emphasized that DBS had no significant effect on such cognitive clinical tests (see table 2) despite levodopa withdrawal in both conditions of stimulation for most of the patients (51/56) at 3 months and all patients at 12 months. The contribution of dopaminergic dysfunction to these frontal lobe–like deficits is, however, not clear. Levodopa has been reported to improve,33 impair,34 or not affect35,36 frontal cognitive performance of parkinsonian patients, depending on the tests used and the selection of patients.37 Different neural networks are probably involved in the various tasks. Some of these networks might be unaffected by the dopamine deficiency, but also by stimulation of the STN or the GPi. The level of difficulty of the task may also play a role. For instance, in spatial working memory the effect of stimulation was observed for eight but not for six boxes. The cognitive deficits of patients selected for DBS were very limited (see tables 2 and 4⇑). Ceiling effects may explain the lack of influence of DBS on tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (see table 2) or Attentional Set Shifting (see table 3).
The results shown in table 3 suggest a greater improvement under stimulation for STN patients, but there was no group effect and no interaction between group and stimulation condition given high between-subject variability. This is surprising given recent models of frontostriatal circuitry suggesting that the direct pathway between the putamen and the GPi and the indirect pathway from the STN to the GPi via the GPe produce contrasting effects with facilitation or suppression of cortical activity.38 However, the number of patients with GPi stimulation may have been insufficient to allow the detection of such contrasting effects. Alternatively, such models may be questioned: dendritic domains in the STN and the GPi are quite large and the axonal plexi of afferents span territories far beyond the proposed independent circuits.39
By comparison with the preoperative state, there was no cognitive decrease at 12 months postsurgery in attention, construction, initiation, conceptualization, or memory scores on the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale, in verbal learning immediate or delayed free or cued recall, in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (number of criteria, perseverative errors or abandons), in graphic or motor series, or in the Stroop or the Trail Making Test, whichever the condition. The single long-term decrease in performance was observed in category fluency at 3 and 12 months in STN patients, with a trend for a similar deficit in literal fluency. Previously observed in DBS of the GPi,11 the deficit in category fluency persisted 12 months after surgery in pallidotomy.4 It might be a consequence of surgery, related to the parasagittal trajectory close to the anterior cingulate cortex used for electrode implantation. Indeed, functional activity of paracingulate and cingulate sulci increased during word generation in an fMRI study.40 It might be argued that the circuits underlying this task are decommissioned by the surgery and that a whole new circuit is brought into play.41 That new circuit would be untouched by DBS, because it was not modified by the stimulation state (stimulator “on” or “off”). From a cognitive or behavioral point of view, this lexical fluency deficit is difficult to explain. It is not related to a cognitive or phonologic slowing because there was an increase in performance in the Trail Making and Stroop Tests after surgery. Neither is it related to a mood change, as there was a mild but significant improvement of mood on the Beck Depression Inventory after surgery. It may be suggested that lexical fluency is less externally guided than other tests of executive functions and requires greater self-initiation. It would therefore be more sensitive to subtle personality changes, such as a decrease in self-activation.42 Behavioral scales would be necessary to determine such subtleties and would allow this question to be answered.4
Acknowledgments
Supported by INSERM and the National Parkinson Foundation (Miami).
Acknowledgment
Drs. P. Cornu, D. Dormont, B. Pidoux, A. Benazzouz, A. Koudsie, and J.F. Le Bas, and the nurses of the “Centre d’Investigation Clinique,” are thanked for their contribution.
- Received January 4, 2000.
- Accepted April 20, 2000.
References
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Trepanier LP, Saint-Cyr JA, Lozano AM, Lang AE. Neuropsychological consequences of posteroventral pallidotomy for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 1998;51:207–215.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Krack P, Pollak P, Limousin P, et al. Subthalamic nucleus or internal pallidum stimulation in young onset Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1998;121:451–457.
- ↵
Kumar R, Lozano AM, Montgomery E, Lang AE. Pallidotomy and deep brain stimulation of the pallidum and subthalamic nucleus in advanced Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 1998;13 (S1):73–82.
- ↵
- ↵
Tröster AI, Fields JA, Wilkinson SB, et al. Unilateral pallidal stimulation for Parkinson’s disease: neurobehavioral functioning before and 3 months after electrode implantation. Neurology 1997;49:1078–1083.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Doherty SM, Jordan N, Tidswell P, Sullivan EV. Different effects of dopaminergic and anticholinergic therapies on cognitive and motor function in Parkinson’s disease. A follow-up study of untreated patients. Brain 1992;115:1701–1725.
- ↵
Fahn S, Elton RL, UPDRS Development Committee. Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Calne D, Golstein M, eds. Recent developments in Parkinson’s disease. Florham Park, NY: MacMillan Healthcare Information, 1987;2:153–163, 293–304.
- ↵
Bejjani B, Damier P, Arnulf I, et al. Pallidal stimulation for Parkinson’s disease. Two targets? Neurology 1997;49:1564–1569.
- ↵
- ↵
Cardebat D, Doyon B, Puel M, Goulet P, Joanette Y. Evocation lexicale formelle et sémantique chez des sujets normaux. Performances et dynamiques de production en fonction du sexe, de l’âge et du niveau d’étude. Acta Neurol Belge 1990;90:207–217.
- ↵
Luria AR. Higher cortical functions in man. New York, NY:Basic Books Inc. Publishers, 1966.
- ↵
Golden CJ. Stroop color and word test. Chicago:Stoelting Company, 1978.
- ↵
- ↵
Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL:Psychological Assessment Resources Inc., 1988.
- ↵
Grober E, Buschke H. Genuine memory deficits in dementia. Dev Neuropsychol 1987;3:13–36.
- ↵
Pillon B, Deweer B, Michon A, Malapani C, Agid Y, Dubois B. Are explicit memory disorders of progressive supranuclear palsy related to damage of striato-frontal circuits? Comparison with Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s, and Huntington’s diseases. Neurology 1994;44:1264–1270.
- ↵
Beck AT. Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX:The Psychological Corporation, 1987.
- ↵
Jahanshahi M, Brown RG, Marsden CD. The effect of withdrawal of dopaminergic medication on simple and choice reaction time and the use of advance information in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992;55:1168–1176.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Ewans AC, Meyer E. Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1993;90:873–877.
- ↵
- ↵
Bowen FP, Kamieny RS, Burns MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: effects of levodopa on concept formation. Neurology 1975;25:701–704.
- ↵
Gotham AM, Brown RG, Marsden CD. ‘Frontal’ cognitive function in patients with Parkinson’s disease ‘on’ and ‘off’ levodopa. Brain 1988;111:299–321.
- ↵
Pillon B, Dubois B, Bonnet AM, et al. Cognitive “slowing” in Parkinson’s disease fails to respond to levodopa treatment: “The 15 objects test.” Neurology 1989;39:762–768.
- ↵
Growdon JH, Kieburtz K, McDermont MP, Panisset M, Friedman JH. Levodopa improves motor function without impairing cognition in mild non-demented Parkinson’s disease patients. Neurology 1998;50:1327–1331.
- ↵
Kulisevsky J, Avila A, Barbanoj M, Berthier ML, Gironell A. Acute effects of levodopa on neuropsychological performance in stable and fluctuating Parkinson’s disease patients at different levodopa plasma levels. Brain 1996;119:2121–2132.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Crosson B, Sadek JR, Bobholz J, et al. Activity in the paracingulate and cingulate sulci during word generation: an fMRI study of functional anatomy. Cereb Cortex 1999;9:307–316.
- ↵
Marsden CD, Obeso JA. The functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of stereotaxic surgery in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1994;117:877–897.
- ↵
Laplane D, Levasseur M, Pillon B, et al. Obsessive-compulsive and other behavioral changes with bilateral basal ganglia lesions. A neuropsychological, magnetic resonance imaging and positron tomography study. Brain 1989;112:699–725.
Letters: Rapid online correspondence
REQUIREMENTS
You must ensure that your Disclosures have been updated within the previous six months. Please go to our Submission Site to add or update your Disclosure information.
Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.
If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.
Submission specifications:
- Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
- Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
- Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
- Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
- Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.
You May Also be Interested in
Dr. Babak Hooshmand and Dr. David Smith
► Watch
Related Articles
- No related articles found.
Alert Me
Recommended articles
-
Articles
Comparative effects of unilateral and bilateral subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulationR. Kumar, A.M. Lozano, E. Sime et al.Neurology, August 01, 1999 -
Articles
Randomized trial of deep brain stimulation for Parkinson diseaseThirty-six-month outcomesFrances M. Weaver, Kenneth A. Follett, Matthew Stern et al.Neurology, June 20, 2012 -
Expedited Publication
Double-blind evaluation of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation in advanced Parkinson's diseaseR. Kumar, A. M. Lozano, Y. J. Kim et al.Neurology, September 01, 1998 -
Views and Reviews
A meta-regression of the long-term effects of deep brain stimulation on balance and gait in PDR.J. St. George, J.G. Nutt, K.J. Burchiel et al.Neurology, October 04, 2010