Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion (DEI)
    • Innovations in Care Delivery
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit New Manuscript
    • Submit Revised Manuscript
    • Author Center

Advanced Search

Main menu

  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion (DEI)
    • Innovations in Care Delivery
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit New Manuscript
    • Submit Revised Manuscript
    • Author Center
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Neurology
Home
The most widely read and highly cited peer-reviewed neurology journal
  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
Site Logo
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

Share

June 12, 2001; 56 (suppl 4) Articles

The influence of right frontotemporal dysfunction on social behavior in frontotemporal dementia

P. Mychack, J. H. Kramer, K. B. Boone, B. L. Miller
First published June 12, 2001, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.56.suppl_4.S11
P. Mychack
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
J. H. Kramer
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
K. B. Boone
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
B. L. Miller
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Full PDF
Citation
The influence of right frontotemporal dysfunction on social behavior in frontotemporal dementia
P. Mychack, J. H. Kramer, K. B. Boone, B. L. Miller
Neurology Jun 2001, 56 (suppl 4) S11-S15; DOI: 10.1212/WNL.56.suppl_4.S11

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Permissions

Make Comment

See Comments

Downloads
1141

Share

  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Loading

Abstract

Background: Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is associated with a variety of cognitive and behavioral dysfunctions. Symptoms may be influenced by the relative involvement of the right versus the left hemisphere, with left-sided FTD manifesting language changes and right-sided FTD presenting with aggressive, antisocial, and other socially undesirable behaviors.

Objective: To test the hypothesis that right-sided FTD is associated with socially undesirable behavior.

Methods: The authors assessed 41 patients with FTD diagnosed by the new research criteria for FTD1 including behavioral, neuropsychologic, and neurologic testing as well as SPECT and MRI. Based on visual inspection of SPECT scans, 12 patients were classified as having predominantly right-sided and 19 patients were classified as having predominantly left-sided FTD. A clinician blinded to the imaging data reviewed medical records to tabulate the frequency of the following socially undesirable behaviors: criminal behavior, aggression, loss of job, alienation from family/friends, financial recklessness, sexually deviant behavior, and abnormal response to spousal crisis.

Results: Eleven of 12 right-sided and 2 of 19 left-sided FTD patients had socially undesirable behavior as an early presenting symptom (χ = 23.3, p < 0.001).

Conclusion: The authors conclude that right-sided frontotemporal degeneration is associated with socially undesirable behavior. The early presence of socially undesirable behavior in FTD differentiates right-sided from left-sided degeneration. The results highlight the importance of the right hemisphere, especially frontotemporal regions, in the mediation of social behavior. The potential mechanism for these social losses with right-sided disease is discussed.

Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) refers to a clinical syndrome first described more than 100 years ago by Arnold Pick.2 It is caused by selective degeneration of the frontal and anterior temporal lobes.3,4⇓ FTD represents a heterogeneous group of disorders with variable clinical and pathologic manifestations. FTD is dominated by prominent and often disturbing behavioral manifestations, and nearly every abnormal behavior found in primates with lesions in the anterior frontotemporal regions has been described in humans with FTD.5 Highlighting the importance of the frontal and temporal lobes in the modulation of mood and behavior, common misdiagnoses for FTD include manic-depressive illness, schizophrenia, depression, hypochondriasis, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and sociopathy.6 Research criteria list the following core diagnostic features: early loss of personal and social awareness; early decline in social interpersonal conduct; impaired regulation of personal conduct; emotional blunting; and early loss of insight. Thus, a decline in social conduct and changes in personality and emotions dominate the presentation of FTD, especially in the early stages of the disease.

We suggest that the constellation of symptoms found in an individual patient with FTD is caused by the separate and additive effects of right and left frontal and temporal pathology. In patients with selective left frontal hypoperfusion on SPECT, nonfluent aphasia is an early and prominent symptom, with intact social graces and no evidence of behavioral disinhibition. Self-deprecation, depression, and social withdrawal are common. With predominantly right frontal dysfunction, marked behavioral alteration with poor impulse control and childish, silly, poorly modulated affect emerges. These patients often become highly critical of others, and their appeal to others diminishes. In patients with selective left temporal hypoperfusion, linguistic deficits are the hallmark feature, whereas social behavior is usually intact. Patients in whom the left anterior temporal lobe selectively degenerates show striking losses of semantic knowledge. These patients have what has been coined a semantic dementia, which has proven to be a powerful model for exploring the brain localization for words and facts7,8⇓. In patients with predominantly right temporal dysfunction, however, the clinical presentation is dominated by behavioral disorders, including irritability, impulsiveness, bizarre alterations in dress, limited and fixed ideas, decreased facial expression, and increased visual alertness.9 Furthermore, alterations in self-concept lead to changes in political convictions, religious ideology, and social loyalties.

Thus, FTD is not a unitary clinical disorder that is easily diagnosed based on simple cognitive formulas. Rather, it is a rich group of clinical disorders characterized by variable, often asymmetric, degeneration of the anterior frontal and temporal lobes. This asymmetry contributes to the wide variety of clinical and neuropsychologic presentations of FTD. The accurate recognition and diagnosis of FTD requires a different approach to assessment as diagnosis requires a rigorous quantification of behavior and a determination of how the patient’s behavior has changed. The purpose of the current study was to compare the early symptoms of patients with predominantly left-sided versus right-sided FTD, specifically looking for the presence of socially undesirable behavior. The goal was to determine whether an association exists between laterality and socially undesirable behavior.

Methods.

Patients.

Forty-one patients with clinically diagnosed frontotemporal dementia received neurobehavioral, neuropsychologic, and imaging studies as previously described.9 Patients were seen through the UCLA AD Center. All patients met the research criteria established for FTD.1 All patients were right-handed.

Classification of laterality.

Cranial MRI and SPECT were performed on all patients. SPECT was performed with 133Xe,10 which gave absolute measures of regional cerebral bloodflow (rCBF), and 99mTc-hexamethyl-propyleneamine-oxime11 (HMPAO) for high resolution qualitative images. SPECT images of the 41 patients were rated by two independent clinicians who were blind to patient names. A consensus classification of laterality was made in 31 of the patients based on visual inspection. Twelve patients were classified as predominantly right-sided, and 19 were classified as predominantly left-sided.

Socially undesirable behaviors.

The medical records of the 31 patients were reviewed by a clinician (PM) who was blind to the imaging data. Medical records included extensive interviews with the patient and multiple family members. Interviews took place over several days and included structured and semi-structured interviews. In addition, observational data were collected on each patient, as reported by multiple clinicians during the course of the evaluation. As part of the evaluation, patients and their families were queried for the presence of current or past history of the following specific socially undesirable behaviors: criminal behavior, aggression, loss of job (for behavioral, and not cognitive, reasons), alienation from family/friends, financial recklessness, sexually deviant behavior, and abnormal response to spousal crisis. The frequency of these specific behaviors was then tabulated for each patient.

Results.

Eleven of 12 right-sided and 2 of 19 left-sided FTD patients presented with socially undesirable behavior as an initial symptom. The resulting χ2 was highly significant (χ = 23.3, p < 0.001). The tabletabulates the frequency of the socially undesirable behaviors for each of the 12 patients with right-sided FTD.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Socially undesirable behaviors of patients with right-sided FTD

Discussion.

This study supports the relationship between right-sided FTD and socially undesirable behavior and suggests that frontotemporal regions of the right hemisphere are critical for normal social conduct. The maintenance of appropriate social behavior, however, is a very complex process with many contributing factors. Nevertheless, several areas of research seem particularly promising for explaining the prominent and often disturbing behavioral manifestations observed in patients with right-sided FTD. These include research on the dominance of the right hemisphere in emotional processing, global attention, self-awareness, and maintenance of a sense of self.

The right hemisphere is dominant in both the comprehension and expression of emotion, with its role having been confirmed with many studies of multiple modalities.12 The dominance of the right hemisphere in emotional comprehension, in particular, has been well established. Studies in brain-damaged patients have showed more deficits in the interpretation of facial expressions after right hemisphere damage than left,13-15⇓⇓ which is independent of deficits in processing nonemotional faces.16,17⇓ Preferential involvement of the right hemisphere in the analysis of emotional signals has also been supported by studies of normal subjects’ responses to neutral and emotional faces presented in left and right visual fields.18 The right hemisphere is also dominant for the auditory comprehension of emotion. Dichotic listening experiments have demonstrated a left ear advantage for processing the emotional tone of natural speech, nonverbal vocalization, and musical passages.19-22⇓⇓⇓ Furthermore, studies have shown that patients with right hemisphere damage are impaired in the identification,23,24⇓ discrimination,24 and comprehension25 of emotional prosody.

The right hemisphere is also dominant for the expression of emotions, primarily through speech intonation25,26⇓ and body gesturing.25 Facial expressions tend to be more pronounced on the left side of the face than on the right side, suggesting more intense involvement of the right hemisphere.27 In subjects with right anterior hemispheric lesions, spontaneous and posed facial expressions are less accurate.19 Patients with right hemisphere damage,13,25,26⇓⇓ and specifically right frontal damage,25 show deficits in producing emotional intonation in their speech relative to patients with left brain damage or normal controls.

Consistent with these findings, a distinctive feature of many of our patients with right-sided FTD is the bizarre expression of affect. Spouses commonly complained that the patient seemed foreign or ‘alien’, with a remote, blunted affect. For example, several of our patients with right-sided FTD had fixed expressions on their faces: one looked as if he were constantly glaring and another looked flat and expressionless, although both reported feeling happy. Furthermore, when asked to pose different expressions (e.g., happiness, sadness, anger, fear), both patients had great difficulty. This abnormal expression of affect made social interaction uncomfortable for the examiners and for other individuals who socialized with the patients. Therefore, it comes as no surprise that patients with FTD with predominantly right hemisphere degeneration have such difficulty in interpersonal interactions. They misinterpret others’ emotional states and, in turn, are misinterpreted. Furthermore, the empathic feeling usually present in social interactions was impaired in most of our patients with right-sided FTD. A common complaint among family members of these patients was a pervasive lack of empathy, with several spouses describing family crises during which the patient demonstrated a profound and uncharacteristic disregard for others’ feelings.

The right hemisphere is also dominant for maintaining a global attention to the environment. Whereas the left hemisphere is analytical and attends to details, the right hemisphere maintains attention to external surroundings as a whole.28 This ability allows accurate reading of the environment and enables a person to react appropriately to a situation, functions clearly necessary for appropriate social interaction and behavior. Heilman28 hypothesized that the right hemisphere directs attention to extrapersonal space (e.g., facial and emotional recognition, route finding), whereas the left hemisphere directs attention to peripersonal space (e.g., reading, writing, and praxis). Miller29 et al. studied a population of patients with FTD and concluded that those with right predominant degeneration had deficits in the nuances of social conduct and interaction.

Luria30 was one of the first to describe disturbance of social judgment after right hemisphere lesions. Descriptions of social inappropriateness in our patients with predominantly right hemisphere disease and the absence of social deficiencies in those with predominantly left hemisphere disease provide support for this theory. As examples, one of our patients began placing her head into the car window of strangers to strike up a conversation, another began making open remarks in public about strangers’ obesity, and yet another lost his job after commenting inappropriately on the breast size of women working with him. Importantly, all three patients had been thoughtful and reserved individuals premorbidly, and family members described these new behaviors as strikingly “out of character.”

Research in the area of self-awareness also supports the pivotal role of the right hemisphere in social behavior. Self-awareness is commonly defined as the ability to introspect on one’s own thoughts and to realize the relation of self to one’s social environment.31 Studies of brain-damaged patients with disturbances of self-awareness have shown such disorders are usually associated with lesions of the right frontal cortex.30,32,33⇓⇓ Common consequences of right frontal lobe dysfunction are a lack of awareness regarding alterations in behavior, emotions, and thought processes. Interviews with patients may suggest that all is normal, in contrast to descriptions of marked behavioral dysfunction provided by relatives and caregivers. This was a common occurrence in many of our patients. Furthermore, patients with limited awareness of impairments tend not to be motivated to improve their behavior and often have problems evaluating the consequences and implications of their own behavior. They may fail to see the significance of their decisions for themselves and for those around them. Their behavior suggests an inability to assess the value of each new action, or lack of action, in terms of goals that are not overtly specified in the immediate environment.

An important component of self-awareness is “autonoetic awareness,” a term coined by Tulving34 that is defined as the awareness of oneself as a continuous entity across time. This capacity allows a person to know that the self doing the experiencing now is the same self that did so at an earlier time, and thus is essential for maintaining a robust and consistent sense of self, or self-concept. Patients with right-sided FTD appear to have particular difficulty with autonoetic awareness. A frequent complaint made by the spouse is that the patient is no longer his or her self, or has “lost” his or her self. Striking changes in political convictions, religious ideology, and social loyalties often occur, reflecting alterations in self-concept. Importantly, this “loss of self” often precedes any significant cognitive decline, and it is at this point that the patient often receives a psychiatric misdiagnosis.

Autonoetic awareness also affects an individual’s management of future events, such as personally relevant plans, goals, and expectations. In other words, autonoetic awareness is the cornerstone of self-regulation in that it supports the formulation of goals and the implementation of a behavioral guidance system to achieve them.35 Thus, patients lacking this capacity are driven by irrelevant environmental goals or inappropriate habits or routines.36 Although intimately related to the “frontal dysexecutive syndrome” commonly observed in patients with frontal dysfunction, autonoetic awareness has broader implications for self-regulation in that the “self” serves as the standard against which behavior is measured. Thus, the lack of a consistent and coherent sense of self precludes successful self-regulation.

Episodic memory plays an essential role in autonoetic awareness.35 The right frontal lobe, in particular, is thought to be relevant for episodic memory that connects the recall of personal past, and the associated emotional associations, with plans for the future.33 Recent PET studies have shown an association between the retrieval of episodic memories and activation of the prefrontal cortex, predominantly on the right.37,38,39⇓⇓ Also, a PET study in which subjects retrieved emotional memories from their past showed activation of right prefrontal areas as well as other regions in the right hemisphere.40

However, it is well established that encoding processes differentially engage left prefrontal areas, whereas retrieval process for the same material predominantly involve right prefrontal areas. This observed difference is embodied in the hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry model (HERA41), and the empirical observations on which the model is based have since been replicated many times.39,42⇓ Thus, Craik et al.43 conducted a PET study investigating the possibility that encoding of self-related material might also activate right frontal areas. Their results showed that self-related encoding yielded left frontal activations similar to those associated with other types of semantic encoding (consistent with the HERA model) but also specific activations in the right frontal lobe. The findings suggest that these activations signal the involvement of the self as a necessary component of episodic retrieval, and they may be attributable (at least in part) to the representation of self in this area of the brain. Thus, it follows that damage to the right frontal lobe might lead to shifts in personality and fundamental belief systems. This might explain some of the profound changes in behavior that occur in patients with right-sided FTD and is consistent with family members’ common complaint that the patient has lost his or her “self”.

In conclusion, this study suggests that the frontotemporal regions of the right hemisphere are critical for social behavior and the maintenance of an individual’s sense of self, or self-concept. Research showing the dominance of these regions of the brain in mediating the nuances of social conduct and interaction, the comprehension and expression of emotions, and self-awareness support these findings.

References

  1. ↵
    Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology . 1998; 51: 1546–1554.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    Pick A. On the relationship between aphasia and senile atrophy of the brain. In: Rottenberg D, Hochberg FH, eds. Neurological classics in modern translation. New York: Hafner Press, 1977: 35–40.
  3. ↵
    Brun A. Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. I. Neuropathology. Arch Gerontol Geriatr . 1987; 6: 193–208.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Neary D, Snowden JS, Northen B, et al. Dementia of frontal lobe type. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry . 1988; 51: 353–361.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    Kluver H, Bucy PC. Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys. Arch Neurol Psychiatry . 1939; 42: 979–1000.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    Miller BL, Ikonte C, Cummings JL, et al. Evaluation of the Lund Manchester criteria for frontotemporal dementia. Neurology . 1996; 46: A185.
    OpenUrl
  7. ↵
    Snowden JS, Neary D, Mann DMA, et al. Progressive language disorder due to lobar atrophy. Ann Neurol . 1992; 31: 174–183.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Hodges JR, Patterson K, Oxbury S, et al. Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain . 1992; 115: 1783–1806.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Edwards–Lee T, Miller BL, Benson DF, et al. The temporal lobe variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain . 1997; 120: 1027–1040.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Kanno I, Uemura K, Miura S, et al. HEADTOME: a hybrid emission tomograph for single photon and positron emission imaging of the brain. J Comput Assist Tomogr . 1981; 5: 216–226.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. ↵
    Neirinckx RD, Cannig LR, Piper IM. A new radiopharmaceutical for SPECT. Imaging regional cerebral blood perfusion. J Nucl Med . 1987; 28: 191–202.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    Tucker DM, Luu P, Pribram KH. Social and emotional self-regulation. Ann NY Acad Science . 1995; 769: 213–239.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  13. ↵
    Borod JC, Koff E, Lorch MP, et al. Channels of emotional expression in patients with unilateral brain damage. Arch of Neurol . 1985; 42: 345–348.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    DeKosky ST, Heilman KM, Bowers D, et al. Recognition and discrimination of emotional faces and pictures. Brain Lang . 1980; 9: 206–214.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Etcoff NL. Selective attention to facial identity and facial emotion. Neuropsychologia . 1984; 22: 281–295.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Bowers D, Bauer RM, Coslett HB, et al. Processing of faces by patients with unilateral hemisphere lesions: I. Dissociation between judgments of facial affect and facial identity. Brain Cogn . 1985; 4: 258–272.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Cicone M, Wapner W, Gardner H. Sensitivity to emotional expressions and situations in organic patients. Cortex . 1980; 16: 145–158.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Strauss E, Moscovitch M. Perception of facial expressions. Brain Lang . 1981; 13: 308–332.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Borod JC. Interhemispheric and intrahemispheric control of emotion: a focus on unilateral brain damage [special section: The emotional concomitants of brain damage]. J Consul Clin Psych . 1992; 60: 339–348.
  20. ↵
    Bryden MP, MacRae L. Dichotic laterality effects obtained with emotional words. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol . 1988; 1: 171–176.
  21. ↵
    Ley RG, Bryden MP. A dissociation of right and left hemispheric effects for recognizing emotional tone and verbal content. Brain Cogn . 1982; 1: 3–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Safer MA, Leventhal H. Ear differences in evaluating emotional tones of voice and verbal content. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform . 1977; 3: 75–82.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  23. ↵
    Heilman KM, Scholes R, Watson RT. Auditory affective agnosia: disturbed comprehension of affective speech. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry . 1975; 38: 69–72.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    Tucker DM, Watson RT, Heilman KM. Discrimination and evocation of affectively intoned speech in patients with right parietal disease. Neurology . 1977; 27: 947–958.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Ross ED. The aprosodias: functional–anatomic organization of the affective components of language in the right hemisphere. Arch Neurol . 1981; 38: 561–569.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Ross ED, Mesulam MM. Dominant language functions of the right hemisphere? Prosody and emotional gesturing. Arch Neurol . 1979; 36: 144–148.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Sackheim H, Gur RCJ, Saney MC. Emotions are expressed more intensely on the left side of the face. Science . 1978; 202: 434–436.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Heilman KM. Attentional asymmetries. In: Davidson RJ, Hugdahl K, eds. Brain asymmetry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1995: 228–230.
  29. ↵
    Miller BL, Chang L, Mena I, et al. Clinical and imaging features of right focal frontal lobe degenerations. Dementia . 1993; 4: 204–213.
  30. ↵
    Luria AR. The working brain. New York: Basic Books, 1973.
  31. ↵
    Stuss DT, Benson DF. The frontal lobes. New York: Raven Press, 1986.
  32. ↵
    Stuss DT. Disturbance of self-awareness after frontal system damage. In: Prigatano GP, Schacter DL, eds. Awareness of deficit after brain injury. New York: Oxford University Press, 1991: 63–83.
  33. ↵
    Wheeler MA, Stuss DT, Tulving E. Toward a theory of episodic memory: the frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychol Bull . 1997; 121: 331–354.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Tulving E. Memory and consciousness. Can Psychol . 1985; 26: 1–12.
  35. ↵
    Levine B, Black SE, Cabeza R, et al. Episodic memory and the self in a case of isolated retrograde amnesia. Brain . 1998; 121: 1951–1973.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Shallice T, Burgess PW. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain . 1991; 114: 727–741.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Cabeza R, Nyberg L. Imaging cognition: an empirical review of PET studies with normal subjects. J Cog Neuroscience . 1997; 9: 1–26.
  38. ↵
    Nyberg L. Mapping episodic memory. Behav Brain Research . 1998; 90: 107–114.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Nyberg L, Cabeza R, Tulving E. PET studies of encoding and retrieval: the HERA model. Psychonomic Bull Rev . 1996; 3: 135–148.
  40. ↵
    Fink GR, Markowitsch HJ, Reinkemeier M, et al. Cerebral representation of one’s own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. J Neurosci . 1996; 16: 4275–4282.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Tulving E, Kapur S, Craik FI, et al. Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: positron emission tomography findings. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA . 1994; 91: 2016–2020.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    Tulving E. Brain/mind correlates of human memory. In: Sabourin M, Craik FIM, Robert M, eds. Advances in psychological science. Biological and cognitive aspects, vol. 2. Hove, England: Psychology Press, 1998: 441–460.
  43. ↵
    Craik FIM, Moroz TM, Moscovitch M, et al. In search of the self: a positron emission tomography study. Psych Sci . 1999; 10: 26–34.

Letters: Rapid online correspondence

No comments have been published for this article.
Comment

REQUIREMENTS

If you are uploading a letter concerning an article:
You must have updated your disclosures within six months: http://submit.neurology.org

Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.

If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.

Submission specifications:

  • Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
  • Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
  • Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
  • Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
  • Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.

More guidelines and information on Disputes & Debates

Compose Comment

More information about text formats

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
Author Information
NOTE: The first author must also be the corresponding author of the comment.
First or given name, e.g. 'Peter'.
Your last, or family, name, e.g. 'MacMoody'.
Your email address, e.g. higgs-boson@gmail.com
Your role and/or occupation, e.g. 'Orthopedic Surgeon'.
Your organization or institution (if applicable), e.g. 'Royal Free Hospital'.
Publishing Agreement
NOTE: All authors, besides the first/corresponding author, must complete a separate Publishing Agreement Form and provide via email to the editorial office before comments can be posted.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.

Vertical Tabs

You May Also be Interested in

Back to top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Methods.
    • Results.
    • Discussion.
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Advertisement

SARS-CoV-2 Vaccination Safety in Guillain-Barré Syndrome, Chronic Inflammatory Demyelinating Polyneuropathy, and Multifocal Motor Neuropathy

Dr. Jeffrey Allen and Dr. Nicholas Purcell

► Watch

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.

Alert Me

  • Alert me when eletters are published

Recommended articles

  • Clinical Implications of Neuroscience Research
    Oxytocin and vasopressin
    Social neuropeptides with complex neuromodulatory functions
    Eduardo E. Benarroch et al.
    Neurology, April 15, 2013
  • Article
    Oxytocin for frontotemporal dementia
    A randomized dose-finding study of safety and tolerability
    Elizabeth C. Finger, Julia MacKinley, Mervin Blair et al.
    Neurology, December 10, 2014
  • Articles
    Neuroanatomy of the self
    Evidence from patients with frontotemporal dementia
    B. L. Miller, W. W. Seeley, P. Mychack et al.
    Neurology, September 11, 2001
  • Articles
    Clinical features and pharmacologic treatment of behavioral symptoms of Alzheimer's disease
    Soo Borson, Murray A. Raskind et al.
    Neurology, May 01, 1997
Neurology: 100 (12)

Articles

  • Ahead of Print
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Popular Articles
  • Translations

About

  • About the Journals
  • Ethics Policies
  • Editors & Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertise

Submit

  • Author Center
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Information for Reviewers
  • AAN Guidelines
  • Permissions

Subscribers

  • Subscribe
  • Activate a Subscription
  • Sign up for eAlerts
  • RSS Feed
Site Logo
  • Visit neurology Template on Facebook
  • Follow neurology Template on Twitter
  • Visit Neurology on YouTube
  • Neurology
  • Neurology: Clinical Practice
  • Neurology: Education
  • Neurology: Genetics
  • Neurology: Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • AAN.com
  • AANnews
  • Continuum
  • Brain & Life
  • Neurology Today

Wolters Kluwer Logo

Neurology | Print ISSN:0028-3878
Online ISSN:1526-632X

© 2023 American Academy of Neurology

  • Privacy Policy
  • Feedback
  • Advertise