Dose-related neurocognitive effects of marijuana use
Citation Manager Formats
Make Comment
See Comments

Abstract
Background: Although about 7 million people in the US population use marijuana at least weekly, there is a paucity of scientific data on persistent neurocognitive effects of marijuana use.
Objective: To determine if neurocognitive deficits persist in 28-day abstinent heavy marijuana users and if these deficits are dose-related to the number of marijuana joints smoked per week.
Methods: A battery of neurocognitive tests was given to 28-day abstinent heavy marijuana abusers.
Results: As joints smoked per week increased, performance decreased on tests measuring memory, executive functioning, psychomotor speed, and manual dexterity. When dividing the group into light, middle, and heavy user groups, the heavy group performed significantly below the light group on 5 of 35 measures and the size of the effect ranged from 3.00 to 4.20 SD units. Duration of use had little effect on neurocognitive performance.
Conclusions: Very heavy use of marijuana is associated with persistent decrements in neurocognitive performance even after 28 days of abstinence. It is unclear if these decrements will resolve with continued abstinence or become progressively worse with continued heavy marijuana use.
Marijuana is the most widely used illicit drug in the United States and the western hemisphere. In 2000, an estimated 76% of America’s 14.8 million illicit drug users used marijuana alone (59%) or in conjunction with other illicit drugs (17%).1 About 7 million people in the US population use marijuana at least weekly.1 Because of debate about medicinal uses and legalization of marijuana, knowing whether marijuana has persistent effects on the brain is of interest.
Studies of residual cognitive effects of marijuana following a brief period of abstinence show that heavy marijuana use is associated with deficits in executive cognitive functioning, sustained attention, and memory.2-5⇓⇓⇓ These studies have some methodologic limitations. First, marijuana users were only monitored for abstinence for 17 to 72 hours before testing. Because marijuana has an apparent half-life of 4.1 ± 1.1 days,6 it is difficult to determine if the these observations2-5⇓⇓⇓ were due to drug residues in the body or to withdrawal symptoms such as anxiety or irritability.7 Second, the quantification of heavy versus light users may be problematic. Marijuana users have been grouped by frequency of use2 and duration of use.3,4⇓ When marijuana users are separated by duration of use, it is troublesome to separate the effects of marijuana from differences in age and education (a cohort effect). Third, no structured psychiatric interview was used to exclude disorders like depression,3 which is associated with poor cognitive performance.8
Until 2001, there were no published reports of the residual effects of marijuana use on cognitive functioning after a period of abstinence longer than 12 to 72 hours. In a carefully designed study, marijuana users were grouped by frequency of use and neurocognitive testing was repeated over 28 days of abstinence (0, 1, 7, and 28 days).9 Decrements in memory for word lists were found at 7 days of abstinence but not after 28 days of abstinence. The authors thus concluded that cognitive deficits are reversible after 7 days of abstinence and are related to recent, not cumulative, cannabis use. Knowledge about the cognitive effects of marijuana could also provide a basis for determining the relative contribution of marijuana when used in combination with other drugs such as methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA).10,11⇓
The current study was conducted to determine whether neurocognitive deficits persist in 28-day abstinent heavy marijuana users and if these deficits are dose-related (joints smoked/week). Based on our previous work in cocaine and MDMA users,12,13⇓ we hypothesized that deficits in cognitive performance would be observed only in the heaviest users of marijuana.
Methods.
Participants.
This protocol was approved by the National Institute on Drug Abuse–Intramural Research Program (NIDA-IRP), the Joint Committee on Clinical Investigation, and the Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Institutional Review Boards. All participants gave written informed consent and were compensated for their time. Marijuana abusers were recruited using newspaper advertisements. Participant selection was based on drug use history obtained using structured interviews including the Drug Use Survey Questionnaire (DUSQ),14 the Addiction Severity Index (ASI,)15 and the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS).16
Marijuana group.
The marijuana group consisted of nontreatment-seeking individuals claiming marijuana as their drug of choice who used marijuana for at least 2 years, smoked marijuana at least three times per week, reported alcohol consumption of less than 14 alcoholic drinks per week, and had a urine toxicology screen that was positive for cannabis metabolites at the time of admission to the study. This ensured that all participants were abstinent for a uniform period of time. Participants were still eligible for inclusion if dependent on caffeine or tobacco. Participants were excluded if they met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–IV (DSM-IV) criteria gleaned from the DIS for current or past dependence on any other psychoactive substance other than marijuana, including alcohol, or if their urine toxicology screen was positive for substances other than marijuana and its metabolites. The ASI and DUSQ were used to estimate the number of joints smoked per week and the duration of marijuana use.
Exclusion criteria for all participants.
Volunteers were excluded for past or current psychiatric disorder by DSM-IV criteria using the DIS (i.e., anxiety disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, and major depressive disorder). Volunteers were also excluded for a past or current history of neurologic illness (e.g., head trauma resulting in loss of consciousness, seizure disorder, stroke), an abnormal neurologic examination, or pregnancy.
Data collection.
At the initial visit to the Clinical Inpatient Research Unit (CIRU) at NIDA-IRP, all participants had a medical evaluation, a neurologic examination, urine toxicology screen, and pregnancy test for women. Participants were then admitted to the CIRU for approximately 30 days. This allowed us to examine persistent effects of marijuana on the brain, rather than acute effects. Random drug screens were performed during the inpatient stay to ensure abstinence. No treatment or medications were given over the 30-day stay.
Neuropsychological measures.
The neurocognitive test battery was administered by a trained psychometrician under the supervision of a neuropsychologist (K.I.B.). The neurocognitive battery consisted of tests that assess a variety of cognitive domains. General intelligence was estimated using the Shipley Institute of Living Scale.17 The Shipley estimated IQ correlates with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-R) full-scale IQ (r = 0.79). Measures of IQ are believed to be good estimates of native intellectual abilities (premorbid intelligence) and are resistant to the effects of brain injury. Language skills were assessed using Controlled Oral Verbal Fluency.18 Verbal memory was assessed by the Logical Memory from the Wechsler Memory Scales–Revised (WMS-R)19 and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT),20 whereas visual memory was assessed using the Rey Osterreith Complex Figure21 and the Symbol Digit Paired Associate Learning Test.22 Attention and concentration were assessed using the Verbal and Non-Verbal Cancellation Test23 for both randomly placed letters and symbols. Executive functioning was measured with the Digit Symbol Substitution from the WAIS-R,24 Trails A, Trails B,25 Stroop,26 and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST).27 The Rey Complex Figure (copy), Block Design (WAIS-R),24 and Judgment of Line Orientation28 assessed visuoperception/visuoconstruction. The California Computerized Assessment Package (CALCAP)29 was used to assess both simple and choice reaction times (psychomotor speed). Manual dexterity was assessed using Finger Tapping25 and Grooved Pegboard.30 Participants were tested on the 27th or 28th day after admission to the inpatient research unit. This eliminated any acute drug effects and possible confounding effects on neurocognitive performance from the physical or psychological symptoms associated with drug or alcohol withdrawal. All testing was performed in the morning to reduce diurnal fluctuations in performance. The examiner was blind to the intensity and duration of drug use.
Data analyses.
Multiple linear regression models were used for data analyses. Neurocognitive variables were log transformed if not normally distributed. Exploratory analyses examined the possible effects of age, education, Shipley IQ, depression score (Center for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression), and sex on the neurocognitive performance measures. An independent variable was retained in the model if associated (p < 0.05) with the neurocognitive outcome variable. A separate multiple regression analysis was performed for each of the neurocognitive tests. As with our previous studies that found dose-related effects of cocaine13 and MDMA12 on neurocognitive performance predominately at higher doses, it was desirable to establish a dose-related relationship between quantity and duration of marijuana use and possible neurocognitive decrements. Therefore, models included either joints per week, duration of use, or a cross-product of joints per week × duration. A joints per week squared term was also included in the models to test for nonlinear effects that would indicate a threshold effect. We did not examine the association between frequency of use and neurocognitive performance because 82% of our sample smoked marijuana 20 or more days a month. Interaction terms (i.e., Shipley IQ × joints/week) were also examined. All analyses were performed with SPSS statistical software program (Chicago, IL).
Results.
Table 1 shows the demographic and drug use characteristics of the marijuana users. When taken as a whole, the entire group consisted of predominantly heavy marijuana users (median joints per week = 35; range 2 to 117). The group was also divided into light, middle, and heavy users by dividing the group using terciles of joints per week smoked (see table 1). Except for years of education, there were no significant differences for any of the subject characteristics listed in table 1 (see also below).
Table 1 Demographic characteristics of marijuana users by amount used
Table 2 summarizes significant dose-related effects on key outcome variables for the regression analyses. The R2 total reflects the overall proportion of the variance accounted for by the model after the last significant variable was entered in the equation. The results show both linear and nonlinear dose-response effects (i.e., as joints per week increase, neurocognitive performance declines; p < 0.05). This was found for tests of verbal memory (RAVLT, delayed recall, F[1,21] = 7.30), visual learning and memory (Symbol-Digit Paired Associate Learning, F[1,21] = 6.57), executive functioning (WCST categories completed, F[1,20] = 7.09), psychomotor speed (simple reaction time [CALCAP], F[1,21] = 8.32; complex reaction time–number correct, F[1,21] = 11.96), and manual dexterity (Grooved pegboard–nondominant hand, F[1,21] = 6.55). A significant dose-related effect in the opposite direction (i.e., as joints per week increased, performance increased) was found for the CALCAP–numbers in sequence, false positive responses (F[1,21] = 4.87). Moreover, the models accounted for a moderate to a large amount of variance (19 to 57%) in neurocognitive performance. Duration of use was associated only with a decrease in performance on one test, a test of visuoperception/visuoconstruction (Rey Osterreith Complex Figure–copy, F[1,21] = 4.38). Finally, the combination of amount and duration was not related to performance on any of the tests.
Table 2 Linear regression analyses of outcome variables, demonstrating a significant dose-related effect with marijuana use
To illustrate differences in neurocognitive performance between the lightest and heaviest marijuana users, the group was divided into three groups based on the amount of marijuana smoked as noted above (see table 1). The light group smoked a mean of 11 ± 4 joints/week (range 2 to 14), the middle group reported smoking a mean of 42 ± 18 joints/week (range 18 to 70), and the heavy marijuana group reported smoking a mean of 94 ± 15 joints/week (range 78 to 117) (see table 1). The groups did not differ significantly on age, Shipley IQ score, number of women and men, duration of marijuana use, and alcohol use (see table 1). However, because the mean Shipley IQ score was different for the light (102), middle (95), and heavy (91) users, we elected to take a conservative approach and analyze the group differences using an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with Shipley IQ score as a covariate. The mean performance scores, adjusted for differences in Shipley IQ, are presented in the online supplementary table (available at www.neurology.org). Differences among the three groups were examined with post-hoc t-tests. Comparison of group means shows the heavy users performing worse than the light users on 24/35 (69%) of the neurocognitive performance measurements; this difference was significant on five of the neurocognitive measures. Significant group differences were also found between the light and middle users on four of the tests, and between the middle and heavy users on two of the tests. When the scores of the heavy users were compared to published age-appropriate normative values for each of the tests, scores considered to be clinically relevant (below the ninth percentile for the general population) were found for the WCST–categories completed, Rey Complex Figure (copy and delayed recall), and Finger Tapping (dominant hand).
Interaction effects.
There were four significant interactions involving joints/week and Shipley IQ. A Shipley IQ × joints per week interaction was found for the Stroop (F[1,21] = 10.31). A Shipley IQ × joints per week2 interaction was found for Symbol-Digit Paired Associate Learning (F[1,21] = 8.67), reaction time repetition of numbers-correct (F[1,21] = 5.89), and Grooved Pegboard–nondominant hand (F[1,21] = 8.25) (see table 2). In general, individuals with lower Shipley IQ scores (less than 96) showed decreasing cognitive performance with increasing number of joints smoked/week whereas individuals with higher Shipley IQ scores had fewer decrements and better performance with increasing marijuana use. To visualize the joints smoked × Shipley IQ interaction, joints smoked/week was divided into terciles and the mean for each tercile was used for the joints/week × Shipley IQ adjusted plots (figure, A and B). Shipley IQ groups were formed by splitting the group by the median Shipley IQ score of 96.
Figure. (A) Relation between amount of marijuana smoked2 and Repetition of Numbers Task, number correct for the high Shipley IQ group (squares) and the low Shipley IQ group (circles). (B) Relation between amount of marijuana smoked2 and performance on the Stroop task for the high Shipley IQ group (squares) and the low Shipley IQ group (circles). The lower the IQ score the worse the performance. Both A and B reveal a significant joints per week × Shipley IQ interaction.
Discussion.
In very heavy marijuana users, persistent, negative dose-related effects are found on tests measuring verbal and visual memory, executive functioning, visuoperception, psychomotor speed, and manual dexterity. This effect was nonlinear for some tests, suggesting a threshold effect. Although we find a dose-related association between joints per week smoked of marijuana and cognitive decline, duration of use is only associated with performance on one test and a combination of joints/week × duration is not associated with performance on any test. In contrast to previous findings,4 duration is not strongly related to performance. This is probably because our marijuana group has shorter duration of use (4.8 ± 3.1 years, range 2 to 15 years) compared to other samples of marijuana users (7.1 ± 7.9 years, range 2.7 to 31.7 years).4 Additionally, our findings do not confirm previous reports showing resolution of cognitive effects after 24 days of marijuana abstinence.9 This discrepancy may be due to our approach to estimating marijuana use (i.e., joints per week) in contrast to those of other investigators (i.e., duration and frequency).2-5,9⇓⇓⇓⇓ Indeed, joints smoked per week may be a better estimate of total marijuana intake than frequency or duration of use because a marijuana user smoking 10 joints/day for 10 years would probably show greater neurocognitive effects than a marijuana user smoking one joint/day for 10 years.
Heavy marijuana use was associated with lower performance on tests of memory, executive functioning, and manual dexterity. These findings are similar to the findings of others.2,4⇓ The RAVLT delayed memory test shows a significant association with the amount of marijuana smoked and there is a trend showing that heavy users performed below the light users on all measures of verbal learning and memory. In fact, the magnitude of the difference in mean performance between the heavy and light users is substantial (1.0 to 3.3 SD units). However, because the heavy marijuana group can recognize previously learned material (RAVLT–Recognition), this pattern suggests difficulty with information recall, not problems with acquisition or retention of information. This pattern of memory performance is characteristic of subcortical, prefrontal lobe involvement, and normal aging. Visual learning and memory (Symbol-Digit Paired Associate Learning) are also affected by heavy marijuana use.
There was also an association between increasing marijuana use and decreasing executive cognitive functioning. This is apparent on the WCST and the effect sizes are large (4.1 to 4.2 SD units). Poor performance on the WCST indicates difficulty incorporating feedback to guide and change incorrect response selection. The Stroop test requires suppression of a more habitual response in favor of an atypical one (response inhibition) and involves performance monitoring. Performance on the Stroop is affected by marijuana use but only in individuals with lower cognitive reserves, as illustrated by the significant joints per week × Shipley IQ interaction (see the figure, B). This is consistent with the suggestion that individuals with higher intellectual functions, or cognitive reserve, demonstrate a higher threshold for developing neurocognitive deficits after insults to the brain.31 This argument is supported by observations that individuals exposed to solvents,32 aluminum,33 and MDMA (Ecstasy)12 show similar interactions. Difficulties with executive functions indicate a prefrontal lobe dysfunction. The prefrontal lobe is suspected to play an important role in substance abuse/addiction and dysfunction of this region may be associated with perpetuation of self-destructive drug using behavior and resistance to treatment.34
The heavy marijuana users also showed slower reaction times on a simple reaction time test (CALCAP). However, when presented with more complex reaction time tests, the difference between the heavy and light marijuana users became less pronounced. The reason for this is unclear. In addition, heavier use of marijuana is not associated with less accurate performance except for the Repetition of Numbers task but only for those with lower Shipley IQ (see the figure, A). No dose-related association is found for false positive responses, a measure of impulsivity. Thus, heavy marijuana use appears to be unrelated to decrements in response time, accuracy, or impulsive performance on complex psychomotor speed/reaction time tests. Heavy marijuana use is also associated with lower performance on both manual dexterity measures (Finger Tapping and Grooved Pegboard).
This study has a number of limitations. Despite making multiple comparisons, we used a p value of 0.05 in order to detect small adverse effects of marijuana on neurocognitive functioning. More adverse associations were found than could be accounted for by chance alone. In addition, although our ability to detect more effects might have been limited by the relatively small sample size, significant effects were found on several measures. Moreover, in the regression analyses, the sizes of the effects were moderate to large (R2 = 0.22 to 0.57). Likewise, the heavy users performed two standard deviations or more below the light users on 8/35 (23%) of the measures; this is not a trivial effect. Furthermore, heavy users showed clinically abnormal scores on four of our test measures. Although we use a different estimate of marijuana use (i.e., joints per week) in general, our findings show decrements on similar tests of neuropsychological functioning.4 Those decrements are not secondary to concomitant use of other drugs because participants were excluded for a current or past history of significant use of other substances including alcohol. Although the presence of a dose-related response strengthens the ability to make causal inferences, no definitive statements about causality can be made. This can only be determined with a prospective study of controlled marijuana administration, an approach that would be ethically untenable. Finally, because our primary interest is the determination of a dose-related effect of marijuana on neurocognitive function, we did not include a group of nonusers. We agree with others that a comparison between light users and heavy users is less influenced by confounding variables (i.e., background differences) than a comparison between marijuana users and nonusers.2
It may be difficult to generalize these findings to all users of marijuana because of our strict selection criteria. For example, comorbid psychiatric disorders (i.e., anxiety disorders, major depression) and heavy alcohol use are common in substance abusers. However, we excluded individuals with these disorders to avoid any possible confounding effects on neurocognitive functioning. Finally, it could be argued that the self-reports of marijuana use are inaccurate. The finding of a biologically plausible dose-response suggests that the estimates of drug use were accurate, although this cannot be proven definitively.
The neurocognitive functions most negatively affected were memory, executive function, and manual dexterity. The hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and cerebellum play a major role in these functions. All of these regions are dense with cannibinoid receptors,35 and these results are biologically plausible because tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) has been shown to cause deleterious effects on these brain regions. Our observations in humans are consistent with studies in laboratory animals that find learning and memory impairments after administration of Δ9-THC.36 In rats, morphologic changes are found in the CA1 region of the hippocampus with acute administration of a synthetic THC analogue.37 Damage to the CA1 is also seen after ischemia,38 toxin exposure,39 or traumatic brain injury.40 Therefore, cannabinoids may exert changes in the hippocampus that are similar to those found with other types of brain injury. Changes in CB1 receptors in the hippocampus are also observed in rats after THC administration and are associated with selective deficits in working memory.41 These animal studies provide strong evidence that hippocampal changes might indeed underlie the memory deficits in the current report.
Following marijuana administration, brain images show lower rCBF in the human motor cortex and superior temporal gyrus and higher rCBF in paralimbic brain regions during a dichotic listening task.42 The authors suggest that the increases in rCBF may modulate the intoxicating and mood-related effects of marijuana whereas reductions in task-related rCBF in the temporal lobe regions may account for impaired cognition associated with marijuana intoxication. In 26-hour abstinent marijuana abusers, lower rCBF was found during a resting condition in the ventral prefrontal cortex, bilaterally.43 Thus, when taken together with the evidence of THC-induced hippocampal damage in animals and with the THC-associated neurophysiologic alterations in humans, our current data suggest that THC may exert a significant negative impact on the human brain.
Finally, whereas heavy use of marijuana is associated with decrements in neurocognitive performance, except for a few tests, performance was not clinically abnormal. However, the average age of our group was only 22 years. Given the large extent of the effects, very heavy continuous use of marijuana could produce progressive declines in performance that might reach clinical significance. In fact, because the pattern of performance on the learning and memory tests is consistent with normal age-related declines in the elderly, continued heavy marijuana abuse might result in premature cognitive decline.
Acknowledgments
Supported by the Intramural Research Program of NIDA.
Acknowledgment
The authors thank all the nurses and staff at NIDA-IRP who contributed to this project. They especially thank Regina Hess, BA, for editorial assistance with the final manuscript and Warren Better, MA, for database support.
Footnotes
-
Additional material related to this article can be found on the Neurology Web site. Go to www.neurology.org and scroll down the Table of Contents for the November 12 issue to find the title link for this article.
- Received May 6, 2002.
- Accepted July 11, 2002.
References
- ↵
US Department of Health and Human Services Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration Office of Applied Studies. National household survey on drug abuse. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office, 1999.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Solowij N. Cannabis and cognitive functioning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Austin M-P, Mitchell P, Goodwin GM. Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathology. Br J Psychiatry . 2001; 178: 200–206.
- ↵
- ↵
Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J, Tuchtenhagen F, et al. Impaired cognitive performance in drug-free recreational ecstasy (MDMA) users. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry . 2000; 68: 719–725.
- ↵
- ↵
Bolla KI, McCann RG. Memory impairment in abstinent MDMA (“ecstasy”) users. Neurology . 1998; 51: 1532–1537.
- ↵
- ↵
Smith SS. Addictive drug survey manual. Baltimore, MD: NIDA Addiction Research Center, 1991.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Shipley WC. Institute of Living Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services, 1946.
- ↵
Benton AL, Hamsher K. Multilingual Aphasia Examination manual. Iowa City: University of Iowa, 1978.
- ↵
Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, 1987.
- ↵
Rey A. L’examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 1964.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Mesulam MM. Principles of behavioral neurology. Philadelphia: FA Davis, 1985.
- ↵
Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-R). New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation, 1981.
- ↵
Reitan RM. Manual for administration of neuropsychological test batteries for adults and children. Tucson, AZ: NM Neuropsychology Laboratory, 1977.
- ↵
- ↵
Heaton RK. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1981.
- ↵
Benton AL, Hamsher K, Varney N, et al. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press, 1983.
- ↵
Miller EN, Satz P, Visscher BV. Computerized and conventional neuropsychological assessment of HIV-1 infected homosexual men. Neurology . 1991; 41: 1608–1616.
- ↵
Klove H. Clinical neuropsychology. In: Forster FM, ed. The medical clinics of North America. New York: WB Saunders, 1963; 1647–1658.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Herkenham M, Lynn AB, Little MD, et al. Cannabinoid receptor localization in brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA . 1990; 87: 1932–1936.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Letters: Rapid online correspondence
REQUIREMENTS
You must ensure that your Disclosures have been updated within the previous six months. Please go to our Submission Site to add or update your Disclosure information.
Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.
If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.
Submission specifications:
- Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
- Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
- Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
- Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
- Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.
You May Also be Interested in
Dr. Nicole Sur and Dr. Mausaminben Hathidara
► Watch
Topics Discussed
Alert Me
Recommended articles
-
Brief Communications
Neuropsychological deficits in long-term frequent cannabis usersLambros Messinis, Anthoula Kyprianidou, Sonia Malefaki et al.Neurology, March 13, 2006 -
Articles
Differential effects of cocaine and cocaine alcohol on neurocognitive performanceKaren I. Bolla, Frank R. Funderburk, Jean Lud Cadet et al.Neurology, June 27, 2000 -
Article
Recent cannabis use in HIV is associated with reduced inflammatory markers in CSF and bloodRonald J. Ellis, Scott N. Peterson, Yueling Li et al.Neurology: Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation, June 17, 2020 -
Eye on Practice
Clinical perspectives on medical marijuana (cannabis) for neurologic disordersTerry D. Fife, Heidi Moawad, Constantine Moschonas et al.Neurology: Clinical Practice, August 06, 2015