Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: The Chongqing aging study
Citation Manager Formats
Make Comment
See Comments

Abstract
The authors followed 5,437 people aged 55 years and older with normal baseline Mini-Mental State Examination score annually for 5 years. The mean incidence of cognitive impairment was 2.3% per year. Cognitive activities in both the individual item (playing board games and reading) and the composite measure were associated with the reduced risk of cognitive impairment, while watching television was associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment.
Current knowledge of life factors that increase or reduce the risk of cognitive impairment in older people is limited. Participation in leisure activities has been associated with the reduced risk of cognitive impairment.1–9 We conducted a prospective study to investigate the association between leisure activities and cognitive impairment in a cohort of Chinese elderly people.
Methods.
Subjects.
The study was conducted from 1999 to 2004. Residents aged 55 years and older with normal baseline Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores in the nine randomly selected communities at Chongqing were enrolled into the present study (see inclusion and exclusion criteria on the Neurology Web site at www.neurology.org).
At baseline screening, the following data were collected: 1) demographic data: age, sex, education (lower education [illiteracy and ≤6 years] vs higher education [>6 years]), and occupation (blue collar vs white collar); 2) smoking (current smoking vs no current smoking) and drinking (daily drinking vs no daily drinking); 3) medical conditions: regular use of nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) and comorbidities (hypothyroidism, coronary heart disease, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, chronic hepatitis, chronic renal insufficiency, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, hypercholesterolemia, stroke, and Parkinson disease); 4) neuropsychological evaluation: the cognitive status was assessed using the Chinese version of MMSE with the cutpoint previously defined as 17 (illiteracy), 20 (≤6 years of education), and 24 (>6 years of education) and functional ability and depressive symptomatology were assessed using modified Activities of Daily Living (ADL) and Zung Depression Inventory; 5) leisure activities: the activities were 13 common leisure activities of the elderly Chinese urban people including playing board games (mahjong, chess, or poker), reading (newspapers, magazines, and books), writing, calligraphy or painting, playing music, drama or dancing, watching television, listening to the radio, visiting friends or relatives, traveling, gardening, fishing, and walking and running for exercise. The frequency of participation in a leisure activity was coded as 7 for daily, 1 for weekly, 1/4 for monthly, and 1/52 for annual participation. The score of participation in an activity was calculated as hours spent per week by multiplying participation frequency with average hours spent per time. Composite measures were generated as total hours spent per week in cognitive (playing board games, reading, writing, calligraphy or painting, playing music, drama or dancing, and listening to the radio), physical (walking, fishing, running, and gardening), and social activities (visiting friends or relatives and traveling).
Subjects were followed up annually. Cognitive status was assessed using the same procedure as baseline screening.
Statistical analysis.
In univariate analysis, baseline variables between subjects who developed cognitive impairment and those who did not were compared using Pearson χ2 test, Fisher exact test, a t test, or the Mann–Whitney U test as deemed appropriate. In multivariate analysis, onset of cognitive impairment was assigned as the time of follow-up visit at which cognitive impairment was diagnosed. The association between cognitive impairment and the individual activity or composite measure was calculated using Cox proportional hazards regression in three subject groups: Model A (all subjects), Model B (subjects who had a full baseline MMSE score and did not develop cognitive impairment during the first year of follow-up), and Model C (subjects without comorbidity). All the models were adjusted with age, sex, education, occupation, medical conditions, smoking, drinking, depressive symptoms, baseline MMSE, and ADL scores, and participation in other activities.
Results.
Follow-up and baseline characteristics.
There was a total of 9,341 residents aged 55 years and older in the communities at baseline, of whom 5,437 (58.2%) were enrolled into the present study. The mean duration of follow-up was 4.7 ± 0.5 years, during which 273 (5.0%) died, 493 (9.1%) dropped out, and 593 (10.9%) developed cognitive impairment. The mean incidence of cognitive impairment was 2.3% per year.
The baseline characteristics of subjects who developed cognitive impairment and those who did not are presented in table 1. Compared with subjects who did not, those who developed cognitive impairment were more frequent in women, lower education, blue-collar occupation, current smoking, daily drinking, and had older age, more comorbidities, lower MMSE and higher ADL scores.
Table 1 Baseline characteristics in subjects who did and did not develop cognitive impairment
Univariate analysis.
Subjects who developed cognitive impairment had lower scores of playing board games and reading, and a higher score of watching television than those who did not. When activities were grouped into cognitive, physical, and social activity, the composite score of cognitive activity was lower in subjects who developed cognitive impairment than in those who did not; while no statistical differences were found in composite scores of physical and social activities between the two groups (table 2).
Table 2 Baseline scores of leisure activities in subjects who did and did not develop cognitive impairment
Multivariate analysis.
Among individual activities, playing board games and reading were associated with the reduced risk of cognitive impairment, while watching television was associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment. On the analysis of composite measures, only cognitive activity was associated with the reduced risk of cognitive impairment (Model A in table 3).
Table 3 Association of leisure activities and risk of cognitive impairment in Cox proportional hazards regression models
Comorbidity and preclinical cognitive impairment have potential impacts on activity participation and the studied association. To control the bias, we verified the studied association in subjects who had full baseline MMSE scores and did not develop cognitive impairment during the first year of follow-up (Model B in table 3) and in subjects who had no comorbidity at baseline (Model C in table 3). The findings were essentially unchanged.
Discussion.
This is the first longitudinal prospective study of the association between leisure activities and the risk of cognitive impairment in Chinese elderly people. The top three common leisure activities in our cohort were watching television, playing board games, and reading. The incidence of cognitive impairment was 2.3% per year.
Cognitive activities in both the individual item and the composite measure were associated with the reduced risk of cognitive impairment in our cohort. On the analysis of individual activities, more time spent playing board games and reading was associated with the reduced risk of cognitive impairment. This is consistent with some studies.1,9 Among the board games in our cohort, mahjong accounted for 96.0% of the board game score, and 69.6% of the subjects participated in it. Mahjong, a traditional Chinese board game, is a strong cognition-stimulating activity involving comprehensive cognitive domains (including attention, memory, calculation, initiative capacity, and planning) and social contact, which was associated with the reduced risk of cognitive impairment.5,9 On the analysis of composite measures, cognitive activity was associated with the reduced risk of cognitive impairment, while physical and social activities were not. Our observations suggest that the association between cognitive activity and the reduced risk of cognitive impairment may reflect mental stimulation rather than a nonspecific result of being active.1,3
More time spent watching television was associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment in our cohort. This is consistent with a retrospective study in Chinese elderly people (see reference E-1 on the Neurology Web site at www.neurology.org). Watching television has been found to involve low levels of cognitive activity and to be associated with lower education.10 The cognitive stimulation from television watching might not be enough to compensate for the aging-related cognitive decline. Moreover, in our cohort more time spent watching television was related to a lower composite score of other activities (Spearman r = −0.255, p < 0.001) and baseline MMSE score (Spearman r = −0.200, p < 0.001). This implies that more time spent watching television might be a sign of the preclinical stage of cognitive impairment, which brought the subject to a passive status engaging in more television watching and less participation in other activities. Thus, it is possible that participation in certain activities is a result, rather than a cause, of preserved cognitive ability.
Acknowledgment
The authors thank Dr. Yan Wang at the School of Mathematics and Statistics, University of South Australia, for help with statistics, and Mr. Anthony Pollard at the School of Medicine, Flinders University of South Australia, for critical reading of the manuscript.
Footnotes
-
Additional material related to this article can be found on the Neurology Web site. Go to www.neurology.org and scroll down the Table of Contents for the March 28 issue to find the title link for this article.
Editorial, see page 794
See also page 821
This article was previously published in electronic format on November 16, 2005, as an Expedited E-Pub at www.neurology.org.
The study was funded by contract no. 20015423 from the Science and Technology Committee of Chongqing, China.
Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.
Received January 26, 2005. Accepted in final form October 5, 2005.
References
- 1.↵
- 2.
- 3.
- 4.
- 5.↵
Wang HX, Karp A, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. Am J Epidemiol 2002;155:1081–1087.
- 6.
- 7.
- 8.
Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang MX, Manly J, Stern Y. Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2001;57:2236–2242.
- 9.
- 10.↵
Disputes & Debates: Rapid online correspondence
REQUIREMENTS
If you are uploading a letter concerning an article:
You must have updated your disclosures within six months: http://submit.neurology.org
Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.
If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.
Submission specifications:
- Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
- Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
- Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
- Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
- Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.