CIND and MCI in the Italian elderly
Frequency, vascular risk factors, progression to dementia
Citation Manager Formats
Make Comment
See Comments

Abstract
Objective: To estimate prevalence and progression to dementia of cognitive impairment, no dementia (CIND), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and relative subtypes, evaluating the relationships with daily functioning, cardiovascular diseases and vascular risk factors.
Methods: We evaluated CIND and MCI in the Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging. The neuropsychological battery assessed global cognitive function, memory and attention. Two thousand eight hundred thirty participants were examined at baseline and after a mean follow-up of 3.9 ± 0.7 years.
Results: The prevalence was 9.5% for CIND and 16.1% for MCI. Prevalence rates for CIND subtypes were 1.8% for amnestic, 2.3% for single nonmemory, 1.5% for multidomain, and 3.9% for CIND defined only on global cognitive function. The prevalence was 7.0% for amnestic, 7.8% for single nonmemory, and 1.3% for multidomain MCI. Incidence of dementia (per 1,000 person-years) was 7.63 in the total sample, 21.37 in CIND, and 13.59 in MCI. In MCI, rates ranged from 8.74 in amnestic to 40.60 in multidomain subtype. The highest incidence of 56.02 per 1,000 person-years was found in multidomain CIND. Both CIND and MCI increased by almost three times the risk of dementia at follow-up. Among baseline variables, only previous stroke and impairment in instrumental activities of daily living significantly increased the risk of dementia at follow-up.
Conclusions: Both cognitive impairment, no dementia and mild cognitive impairment are frequent in the Italian elderly (2,955,000 prevalent cases expected) and significantly predict progression to dementia. Individuation of subgroups with different risk factors and transition rates to dementia is required to plan early and cost-effective interventions.
The diagnosis of cognitive impairment, no dementia (CIND) was at first used in the Canadian Study of Health and Aging1 for individuals older than 65 years whose problems with memory or other areas of cognition were not sufficiently severe to meet the criteria for dementia proposed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition, Revised (DSM-III-R),2 but who were judged to be distinct from those cognitively normal of the same ages. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) criteria proposed by Petersen et al. include memory symptoms, objective impaired memory function for age and education, preserved general cognitive function, and intact activities of daily living (ADL) in persons not fulfilling diagnostic criteria for dementia.3 MCI, in its more recent definitions, may involve only memory domain in subjects with otherwise unaffected cognitive functions, or another single nonmemory domain or even, in a broader definition, multiple cognitive domains.4
Precise operational criteria for the diagnosis of both conditions are substantially lacking. Likewise, data on the predictiveness of the different definitions in the individuation of high-risk subjects are scarce. In this study, we sought to evaluate in the Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging (ILSA), a large population-based study, the prevalence of cognitive impairment according to different definitions and subtypes, and to evaluate the progression to dementia in a 3-year follow-up period. Moreover, we studied the relationships of cognitive impairment with daily functioning, cardiovascular diseases, and vascular risk factors, to examine the influence of these variables in the transition to dementia.
METHODS
Study design.
This work is part of the ILSA, a large population-based survey described in detail elsewhere.5 In short, the ILSA aims at the evaluation of age-related functional changes of the cardiovascular, endocrine, and nervous systems, and at the estimation, in a cohort of older Italians, of prevalence and incidence of the following age-associated diseases: dementia, stroke, parkinsonism, distal symmetric neuropathy, angina pectoris, myocardial infarction, cardiac arrhythmia, heart failure, hypertension, peripheral artery disease, and diabetes mellitus. A random sample of 5,632 individuals aged 65 to 84 years, free-dwelling or institutionalized, stratified by 5-year age groups and sex, was extracted from the population registers of eight municipalities distributed across Northern, Central, and Southern Italy, in both urban and rural areas: Genoa, Segrate (Milan), Selvazzano-Rubano (Padua), Impruneta (Florence), Fermo (Ascoli Piceno), Naples, Casamassima (Bari), and Catania. Following an equal allocation strategy, 88 individuals of each sex were included in four age groups (65 to 69, 70 to 74, 75 to 79, and 80 to 84 years) at every participating center, yielding the overall study sample. Informed consent was given according to institutional guidelines.
The ILSA design includes both cross-sectional and longitudinal components. At the baseline survey, cases were identified through a two-phase design. In Phase 1, involving all participants in the sample, a personal interview was conducted to determine demographics and education level (expressed as completed years of schooling) and to gather information on functional conditions, risk factors, and disease symptoms; a physical and neuropsychological examination, performed by specifically trained physicians, and laboratory investigations including hematology tests, spirometry, EKG, and retinography were also performed. In Phase 2, a specialist (geriatrician or neurologist, according to the different conditions) confirmed diagnoses in suspected cases through a direct clinical examination and review of medical records.
The study cohort was identified within the ILSA population after the first cross-sectional study, conducted in 1992 to 1993, that led to the detection of the prevalent cases of cognitive impairment and dementia (baseline survey). Prevalent cases of dementia were excluded from the present analysis. Starting on September 1995, participants were re-examined following the same procedures used at the baseline examination. Incident cases of dementia were considered all individuals who were free of dementia at baseline but developed dementia during the follow-up.6
Cognitive impairment and dementia.
The diagnosis of cognitive impairment was based on results of a neuropsychological battery including the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE),7 the Babcock Story Recall Test (BSRT),8,9 and the Digit Cancellation Test (DCT).9 The MMSE was used to evaluate general cognitive function by assessing orientation, immediate and delayed verbal memory, attention and calculation, constructional praxis, and language. To control for the effect of age and education level on MMSE scores, subjects were stratified in four age groups (65 to 69, 70 to 74, 75 to 79, and 80 to 84 years) and three education levels (5 or fewer years, 6 to 10 years, and 11+ years), with a final result of 12 different strata defined by the possible combination of these two variables. For each group, mean and SD of MMSE scores were calculated. According to the method proposed in the Kungsholmen Project,10 CIND was diagnosed in individuals scoring >1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean of the MMSE.
Memory was assessed using the BSRT. The subjects were asked to immediately recall a 21-unit story just read to them, and then, after the story was read again, to recall it 10 minutes later. Score ranged from 0 to 16. An event-weighted, hierarchical scoring system was used to reward the degree of organization of oral recollection by participating subjects. Selective attention was evaluated with the DCT (score ranging from 0 to 60). The subjects were asked, in a set time (limit: 45 seconds/matrix), to cross out target digits in three different and increasingly difficult matrices, made up of 13 strings of 10 digits (0 to 9 in random sequence). Each line included from 0 to 5 targets. Using the same procedure as above, 12 different age- and education-specific strata were defined for the BSRT and DCT. Subjects with CIND scoring >1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean only on the BSRT and preserved attention were defined as CIND with predominantly memory deficits (amnestic CIND), and those scoring >1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean only on the DCT and preserved memory were defined as subjects with predominantly attentive (single nonmemory CIND) deficits. Subjects with CIND scoring >1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean on both the BSRT and the DCT were considered to have multidomain CIND.
Subjects not scoring >1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean of the respective stratum at the MMSE were considered to have nonimpaired general cognitive function and were considered as possible candidates for the MCI diagnosis. Again, we identified three categories among these subjects with normal global cognitive function: individuals scoring >1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean only on the BSRT and preserved attention were defined as MCI subjects with predominantly memory deficit (amnestic MCI), subjects scoring >1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean only on the DCT and preserved memory were defined as MCI subjects with predominantly attentive deficit (single nonmemory MCI), subjects scoring >1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean on both the BSRT and the DCT were considered to have multidomain MCI. The remaining subjects were defined as those with normal cognition at baseline.
The diagnosis of dementia required a structured clinical assessment performed by trained neurologists and was based on a detailed neuropsychological battery including Sections B and H of the Cambridge Mental Disorders of the Elderly Examination,11 the Pfeffer Functional Activities Questionnaire,12 the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale,13 a complete neurologic examination, and the review of clinical records. The final diagnoses had to meet the DSM-III-R criteria for dementia syndrome,2 the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria for possible and probable AD,14 and the International Classification of Disease, 10th edition criteria for vascular dementia (VaD) and the other dementing diseases.15 Before the survey, to ensure the reliability of the diagnoses, the clinical investigators participated in an interrater agreement study on the application of the above-mentioned clinical diagnostic criteria. The reproducibility of the clinical diagnoses proved substantial (kappa index = 0.82 for the diagnosis of dementia syndrome, 0.80 for the diagnosis of AD, and 0.71 for the diagnosis of VaD).6
Medical information for individuals who had died before the 1995 follow-up examination was gathered from general practitioners (GPs) and relatives. A clinical investigator in each study center interviewed the GPs and relatives of deceased individuals on the possible presence of dementia before death. Information about drug prescriptions, other diseases, hospitalization, and institutionalization was also gathered. Hospital discharge records and death certificate diagnoses were checked. With the same procedure, information on deceased individuals was also obtained throughout an interim follow-up performed 2 years after the baseline survey.16
To increase diagnostic reliability across the eight study centers, and between living and deceased persons, all putative cases were independently reviewed by an adjudication panel of senior neurologists, blind to the final diagnosis made in each center. In case of disagreement, the diagnosis by the panel replaced the original diagnosis. The information achieved in the study centers was judged incomplete by the panel to rule in or out the diagnosis of dementia in 15 deceased subjects.
Study variables.
With the exception of ADL, Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL), smoking habits, and wine consumption that were based on self-reported or proxy-reported information, final diagnoses were made by the participating ILSA specialists, based on the direct clinical evaluation, review of medical records, study data, and standard criteria17,18 when available (table E-1 on the Neurology Web site at www.neurology.org).
Functional independence was evaluated using the index of ADL19 and the IADL Scale.20 The pertaining information was obtained by the study physicians at Phase 1 through direct or proxy questioning. For ADL, questions aimed at the evaluation of the functional independence or dependence of subjects in bathing, dressing, going to the toilet, transferring, continence, and feeding. IADLs refer to more sophisticated tasks of everyday life, such as using the telephone, grocery shopping, preparing meals, doing housework or handyman work, doing laundry, using public or private transportation, taking medications, and managing money. The capacities of a given individual to perform with or without supervision, direction, or active personal assistance were investigated. The degree of dependence was categorized as none or dependence in one or more ADL or IADL.
Statistical analysis.
Analysis of differences in the frequency of categorical variables was conducted using the χ2 test. Student's t test for independent samples was used for the continuous variables. By logistic regression analysis, we evaluated the role of demographic variables on nonparticipation and the independent predictors of dementia in subjects with normal cognition at baseline. Results were expressed as relative risks (RRs) and 95% CIs. Incidence rates of dementia were calculated as the number of new cases divided by the number of person-years at risk. Person-years of follow-up for nondemented persons were calculated as the time between the screening test at the baseline study and the follow-up examination, or death. The midpoint of the interval was used to calculate person-years for incident cases of dementia if precise information on dementia onset was not available. The 95% CIs were estimated on the basis of the Poisson distribution. Cox proportional hazards model was used to test the independent effect of cognitive impairment on the risk of developing dementia, controlling for demographic variables, and including all the other health conditions under study in the ILSA. The role of cognitive decline in the transition to disability was evaluated in two forward stepwise logistic regression analyses, taking into account all baseline variables. In multivariate models, normal cognition at baseline was the reference category. Statistical levels are two-tailed. The alpha level was set at p < 0.05. Analyses were processed using SPSS software (version 12.0.1).
RESULTS
Of the 5,632 individuals of the original ILSA sample, 170 who at the prevalence day were dead or no longer resident at the address reported in the demographic lists were considered as noneligible. In the remaining 5,462 eligible individuals, the overall participation rate, similar for men and women, was 82.8%, which left 4,521 participants. Of these, the MMSE was administered to 3,425 (75.8%; mean age 74.4 ± 5.7 years, 52.4% men), the DCT was administered to 3,153 (69.7%; mean age 74.0 ± 5.7 years, 53.4% men), and the BSRT was administered to 2,967 (65.6%; mean age 73.9 ± 5.6 years, 53.2% men). Overall, the final study sample included the 2,830 participants (62.6%; 53.7% men) with all three tests performed. The mean age of individuals for whom all three tests were available was 73.7 ± 5.6 vs 75.7 ± 5.7 years in nonparticipants, and the mean education level 6.4 ± 4.6 vs 5.3 ± 4.4 years. Sixty-two subjects were diagnosed as having dementia at baseline and were excluded from further analyses, which left a sample of 2,768 subjects (mean age 73.6 ± 5.5 years, 53.6% men).
Overall, 264 participants were diagnosed with CIND and 445 with MCI. Subjects with CIND represented 9.5% (95% CI 8.4 to 10.6), and those with MCI represented 16.1% (95% CI 14.7 to 17.5) of the study population. Among different subcategories, the prevalence was 3.9% (95% CI 3.2 to 4.6; n = 107) for CIND with normal results on both the BSRT and the DCT, 1.8% (95% CI 1.3 to 2.3; n = 50) for amnestic CIND, 2.3% (95% CI 1.7 to 2.9; n = 65) for single nonmemory, and 1.5% (95% CI 1.0 to 1.9; n = 42) for multidomain CIND. The prevalence was 7.0% (95% CI 6.0 to 7.9; n = 195) for amnestic MCI, 7.8% (95% CI 6.8 to 8.8; n = 215) for single nonmemory, and 1.3% (95% CI 0.9 to 1.7; n = 35) for multidomain MCI.
Tables E-2 and E-3 report the distribution at baseline of demographics, vascular disease and risk factors, ADL and IADL in individuals with CIND or MCI, and relative subtypes compared with subjects cognitively normal at baseline. In subjects with CIND, considered as a whole, there was a significantly higher frequency of female sex, diabetes, myocardial infarction, stroke, and deficit in one or more ADL or IADL. Subjects with CIND showed a lower education level in all subtypes, and those with multidomain CIND showed a significantly higher frequency of hypertension and heart failure. MCI subjects, as a whole, were slightly younger than those with normal cognition. Deficit in one or more ADL or IADL was more common in these subjects, with the remarkable exception of amnestic MCI. Again, hypertension and heart failure were significantly more common in subjects with multidomain MCI. No differences were found in vascular diseases or risk factors and functional activities between subjects with amnestic MCI and those with normal cognition.
The cohort at risk for dementia consisted of 2,768 individuals. Of these, 316 (11.4%) died before they were contacted the second time in 1995. All 316 death certificates and 152 hospital clinical records were collected. Information from GPs, proxy respondents, medical records, and death certificates was considered reliable to define the occurrence or not of dementia for 210 of deceased individuals (66.5%). The follow-up procedures were completed by 1,992 (81.2%; mean age 72.9 ± 5.3 years, 52.8% men) of the 2,452 survivors. By entering demographic variables in a logistic regression model with being a nonparticipant as the dependent variable, we observed that nonparticipants were significantly older (RR for 1-year increment of age, 1.04; 95% CI 1.02 to 1.06), whereas sex (RR for men, 0.94; 95% CI 0.76 to 1.16) and education (RR for 1-year increment, 0.99; 95% CI 0.97 to 1.01) proved to be nonsignificant predictors.
The final data used were for 2,202 individuals (1,992 survivors and 210 deceased with reliable information; mean age 73.3 ± 5.4 years, 54% men) for a total of 8,520 person-years available for the analysis. The mean follow-up time was 3.9 ± 0.7 years. A total of 65 new dementia cases were identified, including 6 cases detected in individuals who died between the two surveys. Table E-4 indicates the number of cases and the sex-specific incidence rates (new cases per 1,000 person-years) of dementia at follow-up in the overall study population, in cognitively normal subjects, and in the different subtypes of CIND and MCI. Incidence of dementia (per 1,000 person-years) was 7.63 in the total sample, 5.02 in subjects with normal cognition at baseline, 21.37 in CIND, and 13.59 in MCI. In CIND, the lowest rates were found in subjects with deficits only on the MMSE and normal BSRT and DCT, and the highest in subjects with multidomain CIND. Among MCI subtypes, the lowest incidence was in amnestic and the highest was in multidomain MCI. Incidence of dementia in both CIND and MCI was higher in women, whereas incidence in subjects with normal cognition at baseline was higher in men.
To take into account the weight of comorbidity in the development of dementia, the predictive effect of cognitive decline was evaluated in a Cox regression analysis controlled for age, sex, and education level, and including all the other diseases and risk factors under study in the ILSA (hypertension, angina pectoris, myocardial infarction, heart failure, atrial fibrillation, stroke, diabetes, peripheral artery disease, smoking, wine consumption, cholesterol), together with ADL and IADL (table 1). In this multivariate analysis, cognitive decline at baseline significantly increased the risk of being demented at the follow-up examination. Considering individuals with normal cognition as the reference category, CIND and MCI both increased by almost three times the risk of dementia at follow-up. Among baseline variables, only a previous stroke and impairment in one or more IADL significantly and independently increased the risk of dementia at follow-up.
Table 1 Predictors of dementia at the follow-up examination of the ILSA cohort
The predictive effect of cognitive decline in the transition to disability was also evaluated. At follow-up examination, information on ADL and IADL was obtained for 1,939 (97.3%) of the 1,992 participants still alive. Considering those totally independent at baseline, subjects with CIND or MCI were dependent at the follow-up examination in one or more ADL in 27.5% of cases vs 19.3% of individuals with normal cognition at baseline (p = 0.001), and dependent in one or more IADL in 42.6% vs 33.6% (p = 0.012). Table 2 reports the results of two models of logistic regression evaluating the net effect of cognitive decline on development of disability in ADL and IADL, including the other variables under study and controlling for disability level at baseline. Both CIND and MCI increased the risk of dependency at the follow-up examination. The observed effect was of the same magnitude for diseases such as stroke or heart failure.
Table 2 Predictors of disability at the follow-up examination of the ILSA cohort
Among subjects cognitively healthy at baseline, 33 became demented at follow-up. Table 3 shows differences in demographics, vascular diseases and risk factors, and ADL and IADL at baseline for cognitively normal subjects according to the presence of dementia at follow-up. Individuals who became demented at follow-up were significantly older and with a lower education level at baseline. They also had with higher frequency a previous stroke (12.1% vs 4.1%, p = 0.024) and impairment in one or more IADL (75.8% vs 43.1%; p < 0.001). To evaluate independent predictors of dementia at follow-up in these subjects, the same variables were included in a logistic regression analysis. Results showed that, among baseline variables, significant predictors of developing dementia at follow-up were age (RR for 1-year increment, 1.17; 95% CI 1.07 to 1.27), male sex (RR, 4.70; 95% CI 1.79 to 12.32), previous stroke (RR, 3.36; 95% CI 1.06 to 10.63), and impairment in one or more IADL (RR, 2.67; 95% CI 1.04 to 6.81).
Table 3 Demographic variables, vascular diseases, and risk factors in cognitively normal individuals at baseline according to the diagnosis of dementia at follow-up
DISCUSSION
We evaluated the contemporary presence of CIND and MCI in a population-based study. Both conditions were frequent in the Italian elderly population (approximately 1,095,000 prevalent cases expected for CIND and 1,860,000 for MCI) and significantly predicted progression to dementia in a 3-year period. Demographic variables, function at baseline, vascular disease, and vascular risk factors were differently associated with CIND, MCI, and the single subtypes under study and showed an effect on the progression to dementia at follow-up. Although incidence of dementia was significantly higher in individuals with a diagnosis of CIND or MCI, half of dementia cases in the study population developed in subjects judged as cognitively healthy at baseline. Our methodologic approach might raise the issue of whether changes in brain function that lead to MCI, CIND and dementia could be seen as a continuum, which may reflect a single pathologic process. Studies with repeated follow-up assessment are warranted to confirm or exclude the hypothesis of a linear progression from one condition to another. Identification of people at different stages across the process may also have implications for selective treatment options.
Differences in the study populations and in the diagnostic criteria used may reduce the validity of comparisons of frequency of CIND and MCI across studies. For instance, representativeness and generalizability may vary, considering that some studies were multicenter and population based,1,21,22 some including also institutionalized persons,1,22 whereas others were based on cohorts of nondemented elderly,10,23 on subsamples of population-based cohorts,24 or on GP research networks.25 Also, in the Cardiovascular Health Study Cognition Study, the diagnosis of MCI required a documented decline from a previous level of functioning detected with an annual neuropsychological testing,21 whereas in other studies,22,23,25 MCI diagnosis was based on the criteria proposed by Petersen et al.3,26 The overall prevalence of MCI in our study was 16.1%, close to the 18.8% reported in the Cardiovascular Health Study Cognition Study.21 Our rate of 7.0% for amnestic MCI was comparable with the 5.0% reported in an urban community in northern Manhattan23 and the 6.0% found in the Cardiovascular Health Study Cognition Study.21 In the Eugeria Longitudinal Study of Cognitive Aging, the prevalence of amnestic MCI was 3.24%,25 close to the 3.02% reported in the Canadian Study of Health and Aging without the requirements of subjective memory symptoms and intact IADL.22 Our prevalence rates were 7.8% for single nonmemory and 1.3% for multidomain MCI, whereas the combined prevalence of MCI multiple cognitive deficit type (including single nonmemory MCI) was 15.7% in the Cardiovascular Health Study Cognition Study.21 In the northern Manhattan sample, the overall prevalence for MCI was 28.3%, the prevalence of single nonmemory MCI ranged from 2.1% to 5.2%, and the prevalence of multidomain MCI was 12.1%.23 CIND diagnosis in the different surveys was based on Canadian Study of Health and Aging criteria,1 on poor performance on one or more neuropsychological scales and informant questionnaires,24 or on a MMSE score > 1 SD below the age- and education-specific mean.10 CIND prevalence was 9.5% in our sample and 16.8% in the Canadian Study of Health and Aging.1 The prevalence of CIND was 11.5% in a sample from the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging24 and 14.8% in the population aged 75 years or older of the Kungsholmen Project.10
Differences in study population, inclusion criteria and diagnostic definitions do also apply to progression rates of cognitive impairment to dementia. Depending on the definition of cognitive impairment and duration of follow-up, reported progression rates were 12% per year,3 10% to 17% over 2-year intervals,27 35.1% and 44% over 3 years,10,28 and 35.7% in a 5-year period.29 Higher rates were reported in studies based on clinical series or elderly volunteer selection, rather than on population-based samples. In our study, the conversion rate to dementia in individuals with normal cognition at baseline during the follow-up period was 2%. The conversion rate of subjects with CIND was, overall, 8.2%, ranging from 1.2% of individuals with deficits only on the MMSE to 9.8% for amnestic CIND, 15.8% for single nonmemory CIND, and 19.0% for multidomain CIND. Conversion to dementia interested 5.2% of MCI subjects and ranged from 3.4% in individuals with amnestic MCI to 5.6% in single nonmemory MCI, and 15.0% in multidomain MCI. These data underscore that there are cases in which impairment of cognitive areas other than memory may be detected as first manifestation of dementia, and that the association of multiple deficits may increase the clinical predictiveness toward progression to dementia. Incidence of dementia per 1,000 person-years was 5.02 in cognitively healthy subjects, 13.59 in MCI, and 21.37 in CIND. The highest incidence rates were found in subjects with multidomain involvement, for both CIND (56.02 per 1,000 person-years) and MCI (40.60 per 1,000 person-years). In both CIND and MCI, incidence of dementia was higher in subjects with single nonmemory involvement than in amnestic types.
The involvement of ADL and IADL in the definition of and in subjects with CIND and MCI is controversial. Pure amnestic MCI should not include problems in ADL, whereas a minimal involvement of IADL could be present.4 We decided not to exclude MCI on the basis of performance on ADL. In the older population, ADL may be compromised as a consequence of comorbid conditions, the impact of which is difficult to disentangle from cognitive problems.30 Our approach was rewarded by the fact that in amnestic MCI, the frequency of ADL and IADL involvement was not different from cognitively healthy subjects (22.1% vs 24.9%; p = 0.381 for ADL and 38.5% vs 44.1%; p = 0.128 for IADL). The complex relationship between functional activities and cognition is further confirmed by the fact that deficit in one or more IADL at baseline was significantly associated with an increased risk of dementia at follow-up, even when individuals with normal cognition at baseline were considered and, conversely, that the diagnosis of CIND or MCI at baseline significantly increased the probability of dependency at the follow-up examination.
Vascular disease and risk factors such as stroke, diabetes, myocardial infarction, heart failure, and hypertension were related to CIND, MCI, and their different subtypes. The effect of vascular risk factors on cognitive impairment and dementia has been previously reported.31–34 In our population, hypertension and heart failure showed a consistent and significant association with multidomain deficits in both CIND and MCI. The occurrence of a previous stroke was a strong predictor of dementia at follow-up. It is of interest that this was also found in individuals who, according to our evaluation, were defined as without cognitive impairment at baseline. Approximately half of cases of dementia developed in cognitively healthy individuals. These findings may contribute to our knowledge of pathogenic aspects of dementia, suggesting that factors other than cognition may be identified as predictors of development of the disease in the older population. Stroke and vascular risk factors such as hypertension, in particular, may impact cognition at different time distances from the index event or first diagnosis.34–36
This study has possible limitations, including the fact that results are referred only to participants in the ILSA who completed the overall tests battery and the limited neuropsychological evaluation encompassing only global cognition, memory, and attention. The problem of attrition may be relevant in studies evaluating cognitive impairment at a population level. In the hypothesis that missing examinations is not a random phenomenon, missing data caused by refusals in studies on cognitive performance in the elderly may lead to an underestimation of true frequency of cognitive impairment.37 In our study, nonresponse increased with advanced age and lower education level, introducing a possible distortion in results. However, our data derive from a national representative population-based survey. The study sample was randomly selected and included urban, suburban, and rural population. Both free-dwelling and institutionalized individuals were considered. The use of incident cases arising within a general population allows a major generalizability of results and may provide a more complete picture of the condition under study. The mean sampling interval was of 3.9 ± 0.7 years. Studies with long intervals between the cross-sectional examinations are at risk of missing a relevant number of patients. We tried to reduce this effect also by using multiple sources of information for deceased individuals. A particular aspect of our survey was the opportunity to take into account the occurrence of major age-related diseases in the whole cohort. The assessment of the different conditions in the ILSA relied on standardized direct examination performed by trained neurologists or geriatricians, not merely on self-reported information. Finally, the number of subjects included allowed us to create subgroups with enough subjects to reliably control for the effect of age and education level on performances on neuropsychological tests.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The authors thank Ms. Maria Elena Della Santa for help with manuscript preparation.
Appendix 1
The ILSA Working Group A. Di Carlo, MD, M. Baldereschi, MD, S. Maggi, MD, Italian National Research Council, Italy; G. Scarlato, MD, L. Candelise, MD, E. Scarpini, MD, University of Milan, Italy; P. Carbonin, MD, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Rome, Italy; G. Farchi, MSc, E. Scafato, MD, Istituto Superiore di Sanità, Rome, Italy; F. Grigoletto, Sc. D, E. Perissinotto, Sc. D, L. Battistin, MD, M. Bressan, MD, G. Enzi, MD, G. Bortolan, Sc. D, University of Padua, Italy; C. Loeb, MD, Italian National Research Council, Genoa, Italy; C. Gandolfo, MD, University of Genoa, Italy; N. Canal, MD, M. Franceschi, MD, San Raffaele Institute, Milan, Italy; A. Ghetti, MD, R. Vergassola, MD, Health Area 10, Florence, Italy; L. Amaducci, MD, D. Inzitari, MD, University of Florence, Italy; S. Bonaiuto, MD, F. Fini, MD, A. Vesprini, MD, G. Cruciani, MD, INRCA Fermo, Italy; A. Capurso, MD, P. Livrea, MD, V. Lepore, MD, University of Bari, Italy; L. Motta, MD, G. Carnazzo, MD, P. Bentivegna, University of Catania, Italy; F. Rengo, MD, F. Covelluzzi, MD, University of Naples, Italy.
Footnotes
-
Supplemental data at www.neurology.org
As part of the Progetto Finalizzato Invecchiamento, the ILSA was supported by the Italian National Research Council, with annual grants to each research unit from 1991 through 1995.
Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.
Received July 26, 2006. Accepted in final form February 1, 2007.
REFERENCES
- ↵
- ↵
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Maggi S, Zucchetto M, Grigoletto F, et al. The Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging (ILSA): design and methods. Aging Clin Exp Res 1994;6:464–473.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
De Renzi E, Faglioni P, Ruggerini C. Prove di memoria verbale di impiego clinico per la diagnosi di amnesia. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr 1977;38:303–318.
- ↵
Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. Ital J Neurol Sci 1987;6 (suppl 8):44–50.
- ↵
- ↵
Roth M, Tym E, Mountjoy CQ, et al. CAMDEX: a standardised instrument for the diagnosis of mental disorders in the elderly with special reference to the early detection of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 1986;149:698–709.
- ↵
Pfeffer RI, Kurosaki TT, Harrah CH Jr, Chance, JM Filos S. Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. J Gerontol 1982;37:323–329.
- ↵
Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960;23:56–62.
- ↵
McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939–944.
- ↵
World Health Organization. International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems.
- ↵
Baldereschi M, Di Carlo, A Maggi S, et al. Dementia is a major predictor of death among the Italian elderly. Neurology 1999;52:709–713.
- ↵
- ↵
Katz S, Downs TD, Cash HR, Grotz RC. Progress in development of the index of ADL. Gerontologist 1970;10:20–30.
- ↵
Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 1969;9:179–186.
- ↵
- ↵
Fisk JD, Merry HR, Rockwood K. Variations in case definition affect prevalence but not outcomes of mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 2003;61:1179–1184.
- ↵
- ↵
Wu CC, Mungas D, Petkov CI, et al. Brain structure and cognition in a community sample of elderly Latinos. Neurology 2002;59:383–391.
- ↵
Ritchie K, Artero S, Touchon J. Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based validation study. Neurology 2001;56:37–42.
- ↵
Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Shen C, DeKosky ST. Mild cognitive impairment, amnestic type: an epidemiologic study. Neurology 2004;63:115–121.
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
Hebert R, Lindsay J, Verreault R, Rockwood K, Hill G, Dubois MF. Vascular dementia: incidence and risk factors in the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Stroke 2000;31:1487–1493.
- ↵
Freitag MH, Peila R, Masaki K, et al. Midlife pulse pressure and incidence of dementia: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Stroke 2006;37:33–37.
-
Inzitari D, Di Carlo, A Pracucci G, et al. Incidence and determinants of poststroke dementia as defined by an informant interview method in a hospital-based stroke registry. Stroke 1998;29:2087–2093.
- ↵
Disputes & Debates: Rapid online correspondence
- CIND and MCI in the Italian elderly: Frequency, vascular risk factors, progression to dementia
- Vincenzo Solfrizzi, MD, PhD, University of Bari, Policlinico, Piazza Giulio Cesare, 11, 70124 Bari - Italyv.solfrizzi@geriatria.uniba.it
- Eric Reiman, MD; Richard J. Caselli, MD, Angelo Del Parigi, MD; Antonio Capurso, MD, and Francesco Panza, MD, PhD
Submitted July 26, 2007 - Reply from the authors
- Antonio Di Carlo, Institute of Neurosciences, ILSA Study, Italian National Research Council, Florence, Italy, Viale Morgagni 46/48, 50134 Firenze, Italydicarlo@in.cnr.it
- Marzia Baldereschi and Domenico Inzitari
Submitted July 26, 2007
NOTE: All authors' disclosures must be entered and current in our database before comments can be posted. Enter and update disclosures at http://submit.neurology.org. Exception: replies to comments concerning an article you originally authored do not require updated disclosures.
- Stay timely. Submit only on articles published within the last 8 weeks.
- Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
- 200 words maximum.
- 5 references maximum. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
- 5 authors maximum. Exception: replies can include all original authors of the article.
- Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.