Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
    • Education
  • Online Sections
    • COVID-19
    • Inclusion, Diversity, Equity, Anti-racism, & Social Justice (IDEAS)
    • Innovations in Care Delivery
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • Topics A-Z
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Patient Pages
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Author Center

Advanced Search

Main menu

  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
    • Education
  • Online Sections
    • COVID-19
    • Inclusion, Diversity, Equity, Anti-racism, & Social Justice (IDEAS)
    • Innovations in Care Delivery
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • Topics A-Z
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Patient Pages
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Author Center
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Residents & Fellows

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Neurology
Home
The most widely read and highly cited peer-reviewed neurology journal
  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
Site Logo
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Residents & Fellows

Share

December 08, 2015; 85 (23) ArticleOpen Access

Disruption of posteromedial large-scale neural communication predicts recovery from coma

Stein Silva, Francesco de Pasquale, Corine Vuillaume, Beatrice Riu, Isabelle Loubinoux, Thomas Geeraerts, Thierry Seguin, Vincent Bounes, Olivier Fourcade, Jean-Francois Demonet, Patrice Péran
First published November 11, 2015, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000002196
Stein Silva
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Francesco de Pasquale
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Corine Vuillaume
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Beatrice Riu
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Isabelle Loubinoux
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thomas Geeraerts
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thierry Seguin
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Vincent Bounes
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Olivier Fourcade
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jean-Francois Demonet
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Patrice Péran
From the Critical Care Unit (S.S., B.R.), Critical Care and Anaesthesiology Department (S.S., C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., O.F.), and SAMU 31 (V.B.), University Teaching Hospital of Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9; INSERM U825 (S.S., C.V., I.L., P.P.), CHU Purpan, Place du Dr Baylac, F-31059 Toulouse Cedex 9, France; Department of Radiology (F.d.P.), Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome; ITAB (F.d.P.), Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Science, G. D'Annunzio University, Chieti, Italy; and Leenaards Memory Center (J.-F.D.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience CHUV and University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Full PDF
Citation
Disruption of posteromedial large-scale neural communication predicts recovery from coma
Stein Silva, Francesco de Pasquale, Corine Vuillaume, Beatrice Riu, Isabelle Loubinoux, Thomas Geeraerts, Thierry Seguin, Vincent Bounes, Olivier Fourcade, Jean-Francois Demonet, Patrice Péran
Neurology Dec 2015, 85 (23) 2036-2044; DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002196

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Permissions

Make Comment

See Comments

Downloads
2614

Share

  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Loading

Abstract

Objective: We hypothesize that the major consciousness deficit observed in coma is due to the breakdown of long-range neuronal communication supported by precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and that prognosis depends on a specific connectivity pattern in these networks.

Methods: We compared 27 prospectively recruited comatose patients who had severe brain injury (Glasgow Coma Scale score <8; 14 traumatic and 13 anoxic cases) with 14 age-matched healthy participants. Standardized clinical assessment and fMRI were performed on average 4 ± 2 days after withdrawal of sedation. Analysis of resting-state fMRI connectivity involved a hypothesis-driven, region of interest–based strategy. We assessed patient outcome after 3 months using the Coma Recovery Scale–Revised (CRS-R).

Results: Patients who were comatose showed a significant disruption of functional connectivity of brain areas spontaneously synchronized with PCC, globally notwithstanding etiology. The functional connectivity strength between PCC and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) was significantly different between comatose patients who went on to recover and those who eventually scored an unfavorable outcome 3 months after brain injury (Kruskal-Wallis test, p < 0.001; linear regression between CRS-R and PCC-mPFC activity coupling at rest, Spearman ρ = 0.93, p < 0.003).

Conclusion: In both etiology groups (traumatic and anoxic), changes in the connectivity of PCC-centered, spontaneously synchronized, large-scale networks account for the loss of external and internal self-centered awareness observed during coma. Sparing of functional connectivity between PCC and mPFC may predict patient outcome, and further studies are needed to substantiate this potential prognosis biomarker.

GLOSSARY

BOLD=
blood oxygen level–dependent;
CRS-R=
Coma Recovery Scale–Revised;
DMN=
default-mode network;
FDR=
false discovery rate;
mPFC=
medial prefrontal cortex;
PCC=
posterior cingulate cortex;
PMC=
posteromedial cortex;
PreCu=
precuneus;
ROI=
region of interest;
SMG=
supramarginal gyrus;
TBI=
traumatic brain injury

Posteromedial cortex (PMC) has traditionally received little attention from neurologists,1,2 possibly as a consequence of the paucity of focal vascular lesions in this region and its concealed localization. Nevertheless, recent findings suggest that it could have a central role in the regulation of consciousness in humans. First, it has dense structural and functional connections suggesting a role as a cortical hub,3 an essential property for complex cognitive processes.4 Second, PMC is also one of the most metabolically active brain regions, both at rest and during self-directed cognition.5 Third, a selective hypometabolism in this structure has been reported in a wide range of altered consciousness states such as sleep,6 drug-induced anesthesia,7 or chronic disorders of consciousness.8,9 Crucially, in patients with disorders of consciousness, resting-state functional connectivity with the PMC was found to be correlated with the degree of clinical consciousness impairment across healthy controls and locked-in syndrome to minimally conscious state and vegetative state.9,10

The objective of the current study was to investigate the prognostic value of synchronicity indexes of the spontaneous activity in the PMC assessed in a cohort of brain-injured patients at the acute stage of coma. We hypothesized that in the challenging context of coma, the study of brain functional connectivity from PMC might decisively permit to (1) infer the involvement of large-scale functional networks coupled at rest with different PMC subregions (precuneus [PreCu] and posterior cingulate cortex [PCC]) in consciousness generation, (2) identify the functional connectivity patterns that are specifically related to brain injury mechanisms, and (3) explore the extent to which PMC functional connectivity at rest could constitute an early and reliable predictor of neurologic recovery in this setting.

METHODS

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents.

This study was approved by the ethics committee of the University Hospital of Toulouse, France (“Comite Consultatif pour la Protection des Personnes,” CHU Toulouse, ID-RCB: 2012-A00009-34). Written informed consent was obtained directly from the healthy volunteers and from the legal surrogate of the patients. Clinical trials identifier: NCT01620957.

Participants.

This multicenter study was undertaken in 4 intensive critical care units affiliated with the University Teaching Hospital (Toulouse, France) between January 2013 and February 2014. We compared 31 patients with severe brain injury who met the clinical definition of coma (Glasgow Coma Scale score at the admission to hospital <8 with motor responses <6; 16 patients with traumatic and 15 with anoxic brain injury; age range: 23–51 years) with 10 age-matched healthy volunteers (range: 24–45 years). All patients were prospectively recruited and managed according to standard of care recommendations.11 To avoid confounding factors, all patient assessments were conducted at least 2 days (4 ± 2 days) after complete withdrawal of sedative drug therapy and performed under normothermic conditions. In patients, standardized clinical examination was performed by raters blinded to neuroimaging data (Glasgow Coma Scale12 and the Full Outline of Unresponsiveness13 on the day of scanning and 3 months later using the Coma Recovery Scale–Revised14). Table 1 reports demographic and clinical characteristics of the patients.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1

Population demographics and neurologic outcome

Imaging procedures.

Acquisitions.

In all participants, 11 minutes of resting-state fMRI was acquired on a 3T magnetic resonance scanner (Intera Achieva; Philips, Best, the Netherlands). Two hundred fifty multislice T2*-weighted fMRIs were obtained with a gradient echo-planar sequence using axial slice orientation (37 slices; voxel size: 2 × 2 × 3.5 mm3; repetition time = 2,600 milliseconds; echo time = 30 milliseconds; flip angle = 90°; field of view = 240 mm). A 3-dimensional T1-weighted sequence (in-plane resolution 1 × 1 × 1 mm, 160 contiguous slices) was also acquired in the same session. Monitoring of vital measures was performed by a senior intensivist (S.S., C.V.) throughout the experiment.

Seed-region integrity.

Two PMC subregions were defined as regions of interest (ROIs): PCC and PreCu (figure e-1 on the Neurology® Web site at Neurology.org). They covered the entire area of the corresponding anatomical region defined by the Automated Anatomical Labeling atlas.15 The structural integrity of these seeds was guaranteed by visual assessment on T1 imaging. Four patients (3 with traumatic and 1 with anoxic brain injury) showed significant anatomical anomalies in these regions and were excluded from further analysis. In the remaining participants of the cohort, the T2* mean values of voxels located in the seed region were extracted from the T2* mean images and 2-sample t tests were performed to compare values measured in the control and patient groups; no significant difference was observed between the groups (table e-1).

Preprocessing.

Data analysis was performed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) and the CONN-fMRI functional connectivity toolbox, version 13.p (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn).16 Echo-planar images were realigned, coregistered, and normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute echo-planar imaging template, and smoothed using an 8.0-mm full-width at half maximum isotropic gaussian filter. The head movements estimated during realignment were included in the model as confounding factors. In addition, using the aCompCor method,17 covariates were included in a principal components analysis (PCA) reduction (3 dimensions) of the signal from white matter and CSF voxels. The residual blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) time series was bandpass-filtered over a low-frequency window of interest (0.009 Hz < f < 0.08 Hz).

Hypothesis-driven–based analysis.

In the first-level analysis, a correlation map was produced for each participant by extracting the residual BOLD time course from each ROI (PreCu and PCC, figure e-1). Pearson correlation coefficients were then calculated between both seed time course and the time course of all other voxels in the brain. Correlation coefficients were converted to normally distributed scores using Fisher transformation to allow for second-level general linear model analyses.

In the second-level analysis, we conducted a within-group analysis (1-sample t test) to determine connectivity maps for each group from each ROI. These connectivity maps from each seed ROI from all participants were entered into a 2-sample t test, to identify regions with different PCC connectivity or PreCu connectivity across the whole group of patients and healthy controls. We report regions that survived a height threshold of false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected p < 0.001 at the voxel level and an extent threshold of family-wise error–corrected p < 0.05 at the cluster level. We further explored our data until a threshold at FDR-corrected p < 0.05 at the voxel level. The connectivity value (Fisher z score) between the seed (PCC and/or PreCu) and each ROI was extracted from the seed connectivity map from each participant, to further segregate between positively vs negatively correlated ROIs relative to each seed region. Fisher z values extracted from the ROIs were converted back to correlation coefficients (r values) for reporting purposes.

To specifically address the influence of the etiology of brain injury, 3 additional 2-sample t tests were conducted on connectivity maps from each seed ROI: patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI) vs controls and anoxic patients vs controls and anoxic vs TBI. Only regions that survived a height threshold of FDR-corrected p < 0.001 at the voxel level and an extent threshold of family-wise error–corrected p < 0.05 at the cluster level were reported. Finally, a null conjunction was performed between TBI vs controls and anoxic vs controls. We used FDR correction with p < 0.05 at a voxel level.

Behavioral data are expressed as median and range. Data distributions were compared with Kruskal-Wallis test, and the statistical dependence between 2 variables was assessed using Spearman rank correlation coefficient.

Data-driven analysis.

To provide an additional methodologic control, we included a complementary data-driven spatial independent component analysis18 to decompose data into statistically independent spatial and temporal components19 (http://icatb.sourceforge.net). A standard template of the default-mode network (DMN) was used to identify network components on a subject-by-subject basis.20 SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) was used for the statistical comparison of images. These ancillary data have been used to create figure e-2.

RESULTS

Plasticity of brain connections after injury.

In figure 1, we report group-level results of seed-based connectivity analyses obtained when PreCu and PCC were considered in turn as seed. The obtained comatose patient's topography resembled the one reported in healthy controls, involving correlated brain areas (figure 1, red) previously described as being part of internally directed cognition networks21: medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), temporoparietal junction, and parahippocampal and superior frontal gyri. In addition, we also observed in both patient and control groups a set of brain areas that were spontaneously anticorrelated with PCC and PreCu (figure 1, blue). Such anticorrelated network bilaterally involved the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) and the insulae, regions that are known to take part in networks supporting externally directed cognition.22

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1 Large-scale spontaneously synchronized networks

Whole-brain functional connectivity maps of covariance were computed in both groups (i.e., comatose patients and controls) following a hypothesis-driven approach. Two seed-based structures were analyzed: posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus (PreCu). Color intensity depicts level of synchronicity (red and blue for positive and negative temporal signals correlations, respectively). All p values are corrected for false discovery rate at the whole brain level (p value <0.0001, corrected for false discovery rate).

In addition, to quantify the observational similarity between healthy control and patient connectivity maps, we computed the statistical differences between the obtained networks (figure 2, in purple and green significant positive and negative differences, respectively). When considering PCC as seed region, we observed a significant and specific functional connectivity change in patients who were comatose relative to healthy controls (figure 2A). It is worth noting that these functional connectivity disruptions from PCC involved spontaneously correlated (e.g., mPFC) and anticorrelated (e.g., SMG) brain areas (figure 2B). A data-driven spatial independent component analysis, used as a methodologic control, confirms that DMN was indeed much less integrated in patients who were comatose compared with controls (figure e-2).

Figure 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2 Comparison between intrinsic PCC synchronized networks identified in patients who were comatose and controls

(A) Color intensity depicts differences of synchronicity between both groups (purple and green for positive and negative differences, respectively). (B) Spatial distribution of differences of temporal synchronicity values between patients and controls. All p values are corrected for false discovery rate at the whole brain level (p value <0.0001, corrected for false discovery rate). IFC = inferior frontal cortex; mPFC = mesial prefrontal cortex (BA9); PCC = posterior cingulate cortex; SMG = supramarginal gyrus (BA40).

In contrast, no such group differences were observed for connectivity of the PreCu seed region (p < 0.05, FDR-corrected), suggesting the preservation of a global cortical network orchestrated by PreCu over time in resting state during coma.

Effect of brain injury mechanism.

It is worth noting that no difference was found between the resting-state networks obtained from patients of traumatic vs anoxic origin at the defined statistical threshold (p < 0.05, FDR-corrected). Using a lower threshold (p < 0.001, uncorrected), only 2 clusters of voxel were identified: the left somatosensory associative cortex (BA7) and the retrosplenial cingulate cortex (BA29). Of note, largely overlapping networks were observed when comparing intrinsic connectivity networks from coma patients of traumatic vs anoxic origin (figure 3). A conjunction analysis confirmed and extended this finding (figure e-3), suggesting a common pattern of neural communication disturbance in coma patients, notwithstanding etiology. To summarize, we observed at the same level, in both traumatic and anoxic patients, a significant disturbance in coactivation of distant regions from PCC, in particular with the midline forebrain areas.

Figure 3
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3 Intrinsic PCC synchronized networks identified in comatose patients with anoxic and traumatic injury compared with controls

One seed-based structure was analyzed (PCC) (p value <0.0001, corrected for false discovery rate). Both etiologic subgroups are represented (i.e., traumatic and anoxic mechanism, upper and middle panel, respectively). In addition, overlapping resting-state networks, in which activity is spontaneously synchronized with PCC in both traumatic and anoxic comatose patients compared with controls, are depicted in the lower panel. PCC = posterior cingulate cortex.

Prognostic value.

In this behaviorally homogeneous cohort of patients who were comatose (table 1), we observed that the functional connectivity strength between PCC-mPFC assessed at the acute stage where patients were scanned, was significantly different between comatose patients who went on to recover and those who eventually scored an unfavorable outcome 3 months after the brain injury (figure 4A). In addition, the linear regression between patients' outcome score on CRS-R and the PCC-mPFC activity coupling at rest substantiated a significant link between PCC-mPFC functional connectivity that was recorded promptly after brain injury and patients' neurologic recovery evaluated later (figure 4B).

Figure 4
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4 Predictive role of PCC-mPFC coupling measured during coma state and neurologic outcome

The linear regression between patient outcome CRS-R and early-recorded PCC-mPFC coupling at rest (A) suggested a significant link between PCC-mPFC functional connectivity and patient neurologic recovery (Spearman ρ = 0.93, p < 0.003). Functional connectivity strength between PCC-mPFC assessed at scan time (B) was significantly different between comatose patients who recovered consciousness (REC) and those who evolved toward a minimally conscious state (MCS) or a vegetative state (VS) 3 months after the brain injury (Kruskal-Wallis test, p < 0.001). PCC-mPFC synchronicity at rest was not different between REC and controls. mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex; NS = not significant; PCC = posterior cingulate cortex.

DISCUSSION

Theoretical conceptualizations of the neural bases of conscious processes suggest that both local and long-range recursive processing loops through cortical hubs permit global availability of information throughout key cortical regions; it is believed that such a global neural workspace4 is the substrate of consciousness. In line with this theoretical framework, our findings suggest that the neural underpinnings of the pathologic alteration of consciousness named coma consist of disruption of the large-scale brain network involving key regions such as PCC and mPFC. Of note, we found that acute brain injuries responsible for coma yield a reorganization of existing intrinsic brain networks rather than a complete annihilation. At variance with a previous study suggesting that coma induced a dichotomic all-or-nothing distribution of resting-state networks,18 we found that spontaneous synchronized brain activity as a whole was maintained during coma; yet, contributions of the PCC to the network were specifically decreased. In this challenging clinical setting, we found that functional connectivity strength between PCC and mPFC, recorded at the acute stage of coma, has a significant predictive value of further neurologic recovery. Our results support previous reports suggesting that cortico-cortical spontaneous synchronization may be used as an efficient diagnostic classifier for vegetative state and minimally conscious state8,9 and pave the way for future longitudinal studies aiming to fully describe the structural and functional network dysfunction induced by severe brain injury from acute comatose states throughout the recovery phase.

It must be highlighted that most of the studies that have analyzed the functional contribution of the PMC in patients with acquired disorders of consciousness explored this portion of the parietal lobe as an indivisible structure (PCC/PreCu).8,9,18,23 Nevertheless, a growing body of research has suggested an important structural and functional heterogeneity within this large region. Cytoarchitectonically, the PCC is characterized as paralimbic cortex, exhibiting a transitional cell architecture organized in 2 subregions, one dorsal (areas d23a, d23b, 23d, anterior 31) and one ventral (v23a and v23b, posterior 31).24 In addition, tract-tracing studies conducted in nonhuman primates25 and diffusion-tensor tractography in humans26 have clearly identified structural connections between the ventral PCC and the medial temporal lobes, as well as connections from more dorsal PCC to the mPFC along the cingulum bundle.27 Finally, PCC complex cognitive function has been recently synthesized into an “Arousal, Balance of Breath of Attention” model.2 However, PreCu (BA7) is characterized by a fully differentiated isocortex,28 has reciprocal cortico-cortical connections with the adjacent areas of the PMC, namely, the frontal lobes, the supplementary motor area,29 together with its own major subcortical connections with the basal ganglia and the brainstem.30 Functional neuroimaging studies appear to converge with evidence for a functional role of PreCu involving self-centered imagery strategies and subserving successful episodic memory retrieval.31 Our data are in line with this PMC heterogeneity, since we found significant differences of connectivity changes between intrinsic networks depending on whether the considered seed region was PCC or PreCu.

Regarding the temporally coupled networks seeded in PCC at rest, we identified a 2-fold connectivity disruption pattern. First, we observed a midline decoupling of PCC and mPFC suggesting network sensitivity to changes in level of consciousness. Of note, our results agree with previous reports suggesting a link between internal awareness and the activity of mPFC and PCC,22 as a stepwise reduction of connectivity between these midline structures in different sleep states6 and loss of the normal top-down cortico-cortical communication between them observed with propofol sedation.32 This finding is also convergent with the hypothesis of a dysfunctional mesocircuit,33 which posits that the anterior forebrain function is markedly downregulated in all severe brain injures as a result of long-range cerebral disconnections. Second, we detected in patients a deficit of the normal correlation over time between PCC and lateral parietal cortices (mainly SMG) that have been previously associated with external awareness.34 We suggest that our data could depict breakdown of the dynamic reciprocal interaction between internal and external awareness systems during coma and highlight the pivotal role of PCC for “tuning” the whole-brain metastable status.35

Remarkably, the alteration of coordination over time between PCC and midline forebrain areas was found to be at a similar level in both patients with traumatic and patients with anoxic injury. Converging structural neuroimaging data have shown specific perturbations of white matter pathways between PCC and mPFC in both traumatic brain injury36 and anoxia.37 However, we submit the alternative and complementary account according to which the anterior–posterior disconnection pattern jointly observed in both anoxic and traumatic groups reflects remote and diffuse functional imbalance throughout the whole neuronal system (i.e., diaschisis). Future studies combining structural and functional data will be needed to specifically address this issue.

The measurement of PreCu spontaneous activity synchronicity at rest, compared between patients who were comatose and controls allowed us to identify a robust and autonomous intrinsic network, encompassing the areas implicated in the DMN. We observed that even in this extreme pathologic condition of consciousness disruption, the functional architecture of this PreCu-based network as a whole was maintained. This important point could be contextualized with the experimental background suggesting that the DMN may be needed for consciousness to occur but is not sufficient on its own to elicit awareness.38 It seems that this functional connectivity pattern linked to PreCu spontaneous activity transcends the level of consciousness, and could probably be considered as a physiologic baseline.

Several limitations of our study should be recognized. First, the number of patients is relatively small and our results need to be prospectively validated in larger cohorts. Second, there is a possibility that our fMRI findings could change depending on the time of scanning in relation to the time of injury.18 This important point highlights the interest of early and repeated neuroimaging assessments in this setting, intended to accurately identify and monitor the brain network functional changes that are specifically related to consciousness emergence from coma.

To summarize, the reorganization of PCC-centered, spontaneously synchronized, large-scale networks seems to be implicated in the loss of external and internal self-centered awareness observed during coma, largely independent of its etiology. The level of functional connectivity between PCC and mPFC appears to be related to patient neurologic outcome. Future work should further explore brain intrinsic network dysfunctions in larger patient cohorts, aiming to improve patient diagnosis and early prognostication and enable the development of innovative network-based personalized treatments.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

S.S. takes responsibility for the content of the manuscript, including the data and analysis. He conceived the study and has personally made contributions to the design of the study, the acquisition of data, and the analysis and interpretation of data. C.V., B.R., T.G., T.S., V.B., and O.F. have substantially participated in the data acquisition. P.P. and I.L. have made contribution to the design of the study. P.P., F.d.P., and J.-F.D. have contributed to the conception and analysis of data. S.S., F.d.P., J.-F.D., and P.P. wrote the manuscript. All authors have revised the submitted manuscript critically for important intellectual content and they have provided final approval of this version to be published. All authors agree to the conditions outlined in the Guide for Authors.

STUDY FUNDING

Funding was provided by the Association des Traumatisés du Crâne et de la Face, Institut des Sciences du Cerveau de Toulouse, and Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale. The funding sources had no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of this report.

DISCLOSURE

The authors report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The authors thank the technicians and engineers of the Neurocampus and Brain Imaging Center of Purpan (Helene Gros, Nathalie Vayssiere) and the medical staff of the Critical Care Units of the University Teaching Hospital of Toulouse (Mylene Terrade, Fanny Vardon, Lionel Kheruel, Dalinda Ait Aissa) for their active participation in the MRI studies in comatose patients.

Footnotes

  • Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article. The Article Processing Charge was paid by INSERM.

  • Supplemental data at Neurology.org

  • Received April 8, 2015.
  • Accepted in final form August 10, 2015.
  • © 2015 American Academy of Neurology

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND), which permits downloading and sharing the work provided it is properly cited. The work cannot be changed in any way or used commercially.

REFERENCES

  1. 1.↵
    1. Cavanna AE,
    2. Trimble MR
    . The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 2006;129:564–583.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. 2.↵
    1. Leech R,
    2. Sharp DJ
    . The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 2014;137:12–32.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. 3.↵
    1. Andrews-Hanna JR,
    2. Reidler JS,
    3. Sepulcre J,
    4. Poulin R,
    5. Buckner RL
    . Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron 2010;65:550–562.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. 4.↵
    1. Dehaene S,
    2. Changeux JP
    . Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 2011;70:200–227.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. 5.↵
    1. Gusnard DA,
    2. Akbudak E,
    3. Shulman GL,
    4. Raichle ME
    . Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001;98:4259–4264.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. 6.↵
    1. Horovitz SG,
    2. Braun AR,
    3. Carr WS,
    4. et al
    . Decoupling of the brain's default mode network during deep sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009;106:11376–11381.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. 7.↵
    1. Amico E,
    2. Gomez F,
    3. Di Perri C,
    4. et al
    . Posterior cingulate cortex-related co-activation patterns: a resting state fMRI study in propofol-induced loss of consciousness. PLoS One 2014;9:e100012.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Silva S,
    2. Alacoque X,
    3. Fourcade O,
    4. et al
    . Wakefulness and loss of awareness: brain and brainstem interaction in the vegetative state. Neurology 2010;74:313–320.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. 9.↵
    1. Vanhaudenhuyse A,
    2. Noirhomme Q,
    3. Tshibanda LJ,
    4. et al
    . Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. Brain 2010;133:161–171.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. 10.↵
    1. Soddu A,
    2. Vanhaudenhuyse A,
    3. Demertzi A,
    4. et al
    . Resting state activity in patients with disorders of consciousness. Funct Neurol 2011;26:37–43.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. 11.↵
    1. Menon DK
    . Cerebral protection in severe brain injury: physiological determinants of outcome and their optimisation. Br Med Bull 1999;55:226–258.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. 12.↵
    1. Plum F,
    2. Posner J
    . The Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis; 1982.
  13. 13.↵
    1. Wijdicks EF,
    2. Bamlet WR,
    3. Maramattom BV,
    4. Manno EM,
    5. McClelland RL
    . Validation of a new coma scale: the FOUR score. Ann Neurol 2005;58:585–593.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. 14.↵
    1. Boly M,
    2. Massimini M,
    3. Tononi G
    . Theoretical approaches to the diagnosis of altered states of consciousness. Prog Brain Res 2009;177:383–398.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. 15.↵
    1. Tzourio-Mazoyer N,
    2. Landeau B,
    3. Papathanassiou D,
    4. et al
    . Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 2002;15:273–289.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. 16.↵
    1. Whitfield-Gabrieli S,
    2. Nieto-Castanon A
    . Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect 2012;2:125–141.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. 17.↵
    1. Behzadi Y,
    2. Restom K,
    3. Liau J,
    4. Liu TT
    . A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage 2007;37:90–101.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    1. Norton L,
    2. Hutchison RM,
    3. Young GB,
    4. Lee DH,
    5. Sharpe MD,
    6. Mirsattari SM
    . Disruptions of functional connectivity in the default mode network of comatose patients. Neurology 2012;78:175–181.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. 19.↵
    1. Calhoun VD,
    2. Adali T,
    3. Pearlson GD,
    4. Pekar JJ
    . A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 2001;14:140–151.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. 20.↵
    1. Garrity AG,
    2. Pearlson GD,
    3. McKiernan K,
    4. Lloyd D,
    5. Kiehl KA,
    6. Calhoun VD
    . Aberrant “default mode” functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:450–457.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. 21.↵
    1. Raichle ME,
    2. MacLeod AM,
    3. Snyder AZ,
    4. Powers WJ,
    5. Gusnard DA,
    6. Shulman GL
    . A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001;98:676–682.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. 22.↵
    1. Dixon ML,
    2. Fox KC,
    3. Christoff K
    . A framework for understanding the relationship between externally and internally directed cognition. Neuropsychologia 2014;62:321–330.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. 23.↵
    1. Laureys S,
    2. Goldman S,
    3. Phillips C,
    4. et al
    . Impaired effective cortical connectivity in vegetative state: preliminary investigation using PET. Neuroimage 1999;9:377–382.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. 24.↵
    1. Vogt BA,
    2. Laureys S
    . Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrosplenial cortices: cytology and components of the neural network correlates of consciousness. Prog Brain Res 2005;150:205–217.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. 25.↵
    1. Vogt BA,
    2. Vogt L,
    3. Farber NB,
    4. Bush G
    . Architecture and neurocytology of monkey cingulate gyrus. J Comp Neurol 2005;485:218–239.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. 26.↵
    1. Hagmann P,
    2. Cammoun L,
    3. Gigandet X,
    4. et al
    . Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol 2008;6:e159.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. 27.↵
    1. Greicius MD,
    2. Supekar K,
    3. Menon V,
    4. Dougherty RF
    . Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb Cortex 2009;19:72–78.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. 28.↵
    1. Pandya DN,
    2. Seltzer B
    . Intrinsic connections and architectonics of posterior parietal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 1982;204:196–210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. 29.↵
    1. Cavada C,
    2. Goldman-Rakic PS
    . Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: I: parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections. J Comp Neurol 1989;287:393–421.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. 30.↵
    1. Petrides M,
    2. Pandya DN
    . Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 1984;228:105–116.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. 31.↵
    1. Fletcher PC,
    2. Frith CD,
    3. Baker SC,
    4. Shallice T,
    5. Frackowiak RS,
    6. Dolan RJ
    . The mind's eye: precuneus activation in memory-related imagery. Neuroimage 1995;2:195–200.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. 32.↵
    1. Boly M,
    2. Moran R,
    3. Murphy M,
    4. et al
    . Connectivity changes underlying spectral EEG changes during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. J Neurosci 2012;32:7082–7090.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. 33.↵
    1. Schiff ND
    . Recovery of consciousness after brain injury: a mesocircuit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci 2010;33:1–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. 34.↵
    1. Vanhaudenhuyse A,
    2. Demertzi A,
    3. Schabus M,
    4. et al
    . Two distinct neuronal networks mediate the awareness of environment and of self. J Cogn Neurosci 2011;23:570–578.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. 35.↵
    1. Hellyer PJ,
    2. Shanahan M,
    3. Scott G,
    4. Wise RJ,
    5. Sharp DJ,
    6. Leech R
    . The control of global brain dynamics: opposing actions of frontoparietal control and default mode networks on attention. J Neurosci 2014;34:451–461.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. 36.↵
    1. Palacios EM,
    2. Sala-Llonch R,
    3. Junque C,
    4. et al
    . White matter/gray matter contrast changes in chronic and diffuse traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma 2013;30:1991–1994.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  37. 37.↵
    1. Luyt CE,
    2. Galanaud D,
    3. Perlbarg V,
    4. et al
    . Diffusion tensor imaging to predict long-term outcome after cardiac arrest: a bicentric pilot study. Anesthesiology 2012;117:1311–1321.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. 38.↵
    1. Vincent JL,
    2. Patel GH,
    3. Fox MD,
    4. et al
    . Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 2007;447:83–86.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Disputes & Debates: Rapid online correspondence

  • Tracking consciousness emergence from coma
    • Stein Silva, Associate Professor, Critical Care Unit, University of Toulouse, France; Toulouse NeuroImaging Center INSERM 1214silvastein@me.com
    Submitted April 06, 2016
  • RE: Disruption of posteromedial large-scale neural communication predicts recovery from coma
    • Cristina Rosazza, PhD, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico ???Carlo Besta???, Milano, Italycristina.rosazza@istituto-besta.it
    • Davide Sattin, Davide Rossi Sebastiano, Ludovico Minati, Matilde Leonardi, Milan, Italy
    Submitted January 07, 2016
Comment

REQUIREMENTS

If you are uploading a letter concerning an article:
You must have updated your disclosures within six months: http://submit.neurology.org

Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.

If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.

Submission specifications:

  • Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
  • Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
  • Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
  • Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
  • Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.

More guidelines and information on Disputes & Debates

Compose Comment

More information about text formats

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
Author Information
NOTE: The first author must also be the corresponding author of the comment.
First or given name, e.g. 'Peter'.
Your last, or family, name, e.g. 'MacMoody'.
Your email address, e.g. higgs-boson@gmail.com
Your role and/or occupation, e.g. 'Orthopedic Surgeon'.
Your organization or institution (if applicable), e.g. 'Royal Free Hospital'.
Publishing Agreement
NOTE: All authors, besides the first/corresponding author, must complete a separate Publishing Agreement Form and provide via email to the editorial office before comments can be posted.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.

Vertical Tabs

You May Also be Interested in

Back to top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • GLOSSARY
    • METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
    • STUDY FUNDING
    • DISCLOSURE
    • ACKNOWLEDGMENT
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Advertisement

Topics Discussed

  • Prognosis
  • fMRI
  • Coma

Alert Me

  • Alert me when eletters are published

Articles

  • Ahead of Print
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Popular Articles
  • Translations

About

  • About the Journals
  • Ethics Policies
  • Editors & Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertise

Submit

  • Author Center
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Information for Reviewers
  • AAN Guidelines
  • Permissions

Subscribers

  • Subscribe
  • Activate a Subscription
  • Sign up for eAlerts
  • RSS Feed
Site Logo
  • Visit neurology Template on Facebook
  • Follow neurology Template on Twitter
  • Visit Neurology on YouTube
  • Neurology
  • Neurology: Clinical Practice
  • Neurology: Genetics
  • Neurology: Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Neurology: Education
  • AAN.com
  • AANnews
  • Continuum
  • Brain & Life
  • Neurology Today

Wolters Kluwer Logo

Neurology | Print ISSN:0028-3878
Online ISSN:1526-632X

© 2022 American Academy of Neurology

  • Privacy Policy
  • Feedback
  • Advertise