Effect of intellectual enrichment on AD biomarker trajectories
Longitudinal imaging study
Citation Manager Formats
Make Comment
See Comments

Abstract
Objective: To investigate the effect of age, sex, APOE4 genotype, and lifestyle enrichment (education/occupation, midlife cognitive activity, and midlife physical activity) on Alzheimer disease (AD) biomarker trajectories using longitudinal imaging data (brain β-amyloid load via Pittsburgh compound B PET and neurodegeneration via 18fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) PET and structural MRI) in an elderly population without dementia.
Methods: In the population-based longitudinal Mayo Clinic Study of Aging, we studied 393 participants without dementia (340 clinically normal, 53 mild cognitive impairment; 70 years and older) who had cognitive and physical activity measures and at least 2 visits with imaging biomarkers. We dichotomized participants into high (≥14 years) and low (<14 years) education levels using the median. For the entire cohort and the 2 education strata, we built linear mixed models to investigate the effect of the predictors on each of the biomarker outcomes.
Results: Age was associated with amyloid and neurodegeneration trajectories; APOE4 status appears to influence only the amyloid and FDG trajectories but not hippocampal volume trajectory. In the high-education stratum, high midlife cognitive activity was associated with lower amyloid deposition in APOE4 carriers. APOE4 status was associated with lower FDG uptake in the entire cohort and in participants with lower education but not the high-education cohort.
Conclusions: There were minimal effects of lifestyle enrichment on AD biomarker trajectories (specifically rates). Lifetime intellectual enrichment (high education, high midlife cognitive activity) is associated with lower amyloid in APOE4 carriers. High education is protective from the APOE4 effect on FDG metabolism. Differing education levels may explain the conflicting results seen in the literature.
GLOSSARY
- AD=
- Alzheimer disease;
- FDG=
- 18fluorodeoxyglucose;
- MCI=
- mild cognitive impairment;
- MCSA=
- Mayo Clinic Study of Aging;
- MET=
- metabolic equivalent;
- MPRAGE=
- magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo;
- PCA=
- principal components analysis;
- PiB=
- Pittsburgh compound B;
- ROI=
- region of interest;
- TIV=
- total intracranial volume
Better intellectual and physical activity lifestyles have increasingly been viewed as protective strategies against cognitive decline in the elderly.1,2 While lifestyle enrichment is associated with better cognitive performance, there is no clear consensus on the influence of lifestyle enrichment on ongoing Alzheimer disease (AD) pathophysiology through longitudinal biomarker studies. The primary goal of this study was to examine the effect of lifestyle enrichment variables on AD biomarker values taking into account demographics and APOE4 status, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally, in an elderly population-based sample without dementia.
Several recent studies have examined the effect of lifestyle enrichment on biomarkers cross-sectionally. The use of different cohorts, sample sizes, and constructs of enrichment has contributed to conflicting results in the literature.3,–,10 An important difference we observed across the studies was the level of education. Typically, participants recruited through advertisements and after thorough screening have higher levels of education, which is often the main underlying variable of interest. Since educational attainment occurs during sensitive periods of brain development, it plays a key role throughout the lifespan, and may have a significant impact on AD biomarker trajectories. We hypothesized that the conflicting results (regarding the effect of lifestyle enrichment on AD biomarkers) seen in the literature (see Discussion) may be partly explained by different education levels of the cohort. We further hypothesized that the effects of demographics and lifestyle variables on the AD biomarker trajectories may differ based on education levels. Therefore, in this article we specifically examine the effects of demographics and lifestyle enrichment variables on AD biomarker trajectories in participants with high and low education.
In this article, we use longitudinal imaging data from the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging (MCSA), which is a population-based sample of elderly without dementia. We used the following imaging biomarkers as surrogates of the major AD processes: Pittsburgh compound B (PiB) PET imaging as a biomarker of cerebral amyloidosis, 18fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) PET imaging as an indicator of AD-related changes in brain metabolism, and hippocampal volume measured by structural MRI as an indicator of neurodegeneration due to AD.
METHODS
Selection of participants.
Study participants were participants in the MCSA, an epidemiologic study of the prevalence, incidence, and risk factors for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia among Olmsted County residents ages 70–89 years. The participants consisted of the original cohort sampled from the October 1, 2004, enumeration of the county population, and replenishment cohorts sampled from the 2008, 2009, 2010, and 2011 enumeration. The MCSA uses the Rochester Epidemiology Project medical records linkage system to enumerate the county population11 and samples are randomly selected for participation. We included all 393 participants without dementia (340 clinically normal, 53 MCI) with APOE genotype, intellectual lifestyle, physical activity lifestyle, and at least 2 complete sets of AD biomarker measurements (PiB, FDG, MRI). More extensive details of the MCSA design have been published elsewhere.12,13
Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents.
These studies were approved by the Mayo Clinic and Olmsted Medical Center institutional review board. Informed consent was obtained from all participants or their surrogates.
AD biomarkers.
MRI acquisition and processing.
All participants were scanned on 3T MRI scanners and a 3D volumetric T1- magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) was acquired and preprocessed as described previously.14 Freesurfer software (version 5.3) with a longitudinal processing pipeline was used to obtain total hippocampal volumes on serial MRI scans.15 Total intracranial volume was estimated using MPRAGE scans using an in-house method.16
PET acquisition and processing.
Images were acquired with a PET/CT operating in 3D mode (septa removed). More extensive details of PET acquisition were described previously.17 All PET quantitative image analysis, including quality control, was performed at the Mayo Clinic using the same fully automated image processing pipeline as described previously.16,18 Statistics on image voxel values were extracted from automatically labeled cortical regions of interest using an in-house modification of the automated anatomic labeling atlas.19
Global PiB-PET ratio measure.
A global cortical PiB-PET measure was computed for each individual scan by calculating the median uptake over voxels in the prefrontal, orbitofrontal, parietal, temporal, anterior cingulate, and posterior cingulate/precuneus regions of interest (ROIs) for each participant and dividing this by the median uptake over voxels in the cerebellar gray matter ROI of the atlas.
Global FDG-PET ratio measure.
A global FDG-PET retention summary was computed for each individual scan by averaging the left and right angular gyri, bilateral posterior cingulate, and left middle/inferior temporal gyrus values for each participant as described previously20 normalized by the pons uptake.
Lifestyle enrichment variables.
We used the following lifestyle enrichment variables from the MCSA. These were recorded for the majority of participants at the baseline or enrollment visit. For patients with multiple evaluations, the variables collected closest in time to the MRI/PET scan date were used in the analyses. Details about the individual questions that were part of the cognitive and physical activity questionnaires are provided in our previous work.21
Intellectual lifestyle.
The primary intellectual activity variables of interest included the following. (1) Education and primary occupation throughout life. Education and occupation are highly correlated variables; therefore we used principal components analysis (PCA) to form a consolidated variable (education/occupation score) similar to our previous work21 to reduce the numbers of predictor variables. (2) Midlife weekly cognitive activity (ages 50–65 years) was summarized based on self-report questionnaire21 (supplemental material on the Neurology® Web site at Neurology.org).
Physical activity lifestyle.
We used a midlife physical activity score (ages 50–65 years) based on self-report questionnaire21 (supplemental material). In this article, we created a physical activity composite score by weighting each individual component with metabolic equivalent (MET) scores.22,23 These MET scores assign intensities to physical activity questionnaire items, and allow differentiation of light from heavy activities and exercises. We then again used PCA to form a consolidated variable.
Statistical analysis.
We examined baseline age, sex, APOE4 genotype, and lifestyle enrichment variables (education/occupation, midlife cognitive activity, and midlife physical activity) as potential predictors of longitudinal biomarker values using linear mixed models. In the entire cohort, we initially fit 3 separate models for each of the AD biomarkers: global PiB ratio, global FDG ratio, and total hippocampal volume (data in figure e-1). We then conducted stratified analyses using the median education level of 14 as a cutpoint to investigate differences between high- and low-education groups. In the models for amyloid, global PiB was log transformed and also reversed in sign so that larger values would correspond to lower amyloid burdens. In the models for hippocampal volume, we included total intracranial volume (TIV) as a covariate. We included all 2-way interactions with time in the group of potential predictors. In these linear mixed models, a significant interaction of a predictor with time would indicate that the rate of change of the outcome over time differs according to the value of the predictor variable (nonparallel over time). In the absence of that interaction, a significant predictor would indicate that the rate of change in the outcome over time would be consistent (parallel over time) but shifted according to the value of the predictor variable. We also included interactions with sex and APOE4.
Because of the large number of potential predictor variables, we used a Lasso-type penalization procedure for linear mixed models to select a parsimonious model for each biomarker outcome24 while respecting the need to retain nested terms. These methods are implemented in the R package lmmlasso. This method shrinks regression coefficient estimates to zero for some predictor variables, thereby dropping them from the model. It has good statistical accuracy for prediction and is computationally manageable. Per the recommendations of the developer, we selected the regularization parameters controlling the penalization (and hence the number of regression coefficients set to zero) by minimizing the Bayesian information criterion. Final parameter estimates, standard errors, and p values were obtained for the parsimonious models using standard linear mixed models fit with random participant-specific intercepts and slopes. We tested for the statistical significance of these random terms using likelihood ratio tests, and found that all were necessary (all p values <0.0001). Since we did not find significant associations of lifestyle enrichment variables or sex with biomarker values, we tested to see if we had adequate power to detect important associations. Detailed power analyses are included in the supplemental material.
RESULTS
The demographics, APOE4 status, lifestyle enrichment variables, and baseline AD biomarker values of all the participants and those in the 2 education-stratified groups are shown in table 1. Age, sex, and APOE4 status were not significantly different between the low- and high-education cohorts. The greater mean follow-up time for high-education participants (p = 0.04) was primarily driven by a few participants with large follow-up times. Since the times were positively skewed, we also ran a Wilcoxon rank sum test, which had p = 0.09. Any influence of follow-up time should thus be minimal. The number of visits per participant did not differ between the groups (p = 0.31). Persons with higher education had higher job level scores as well as higher midlife cognitive activity levels, as expected (p < 0.001). The baseline AD biomarker values were not different between high-education and low-education groups (p ≥ 0.1).
Patient characteristics at baseline in the overall sample and in the strata by education
The results of the linear mixed effect models for the entire cohort are presented in table 2. The results of the 3 linear mixed effects models are presented in table 3A for those with high education (≥14 years) and in table 3B for those with low education (<14 years). Figure 1 illustrates the predicted biomarker trajectories in the entire cohort as a function of age. The top panel in figure 2 illustrates the predicted biomarker trajectories in the high-education cohort and bottom panel in figure 2 illustrates the predicted biomarker trajectories in the low-education cohort as a function of age.
Models for predictors of each of the Alzheimer disease biomarker trajectories in the entire cohort (n = 393)
Models for predictors of each of the Alzheimer disease biomarker trajectories: Participants with high education (n = 205; ≥14 years) and participants with low education (n = 188; <14 years)
Longitudinal trajectories for predicted (left panel) amyloid deposition, (middle panel) 18fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) uptake, and (right panel) hippocampal volume as a function of age. In each of the plots, the variables that significantly influenced the trajectories were also considered, i.e., APOE4 carrier status influenced amyloid deposition and FDG uptake.
Longitudinal trajectories for predicted (left panel) amyloid deposition, (middle panel) 18fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) uptake, and (right panel) hippocampal volume as a function of age. In the top panels (A), participants were in the higher education cohort (education ≥14), and the lower panels (B) participants were in the lower education cohort (education <14). In each of the plots, the variables that significantly influenced the trajectories are additionally plotted. High cognitive activity was defined as the 67th percentile, and low cognitive activity as the 33rd percentile.
Models for prediction of amyloid deposition.
In the amyloid models for the entire cohort as well as the education-stratified cohorts, older age and APOE4 status was associated with worse amyloid levels (p < 0.05). In the entire cohort amyloid model, APOE4 status was also associated with faster rate of increase in amyloid deposition (p < 0.001). In the model of amyloid deposition for high education levels, high midlife cognitive activity was associated with lower amyloid deposition in APOE4 carriers (coefficient = 0.0630, p < 0.05). Midlife physical activity was not associated with the amyloid trajectories.
Models for prediction of FDG uptake.
In the FDG uptake models for the entire cohort and both the education strata, older age was associated with low FDG uptake (p < 0.05). In the FDG models for the entire cohort and the low-education cohort, APOE4 status was associated with worse FDG uptake (p < 0.05). None of the enrichment variables was associated with FDG uptake.
Models for prediction of hippocampal volume.
In all of the models with hippocampal volume as an outcome, increasing age was associated with lower hippocampal volume. As expected, larger TIV was a predictor of larger hippocampal volume (p < 0.001). In the model of hippocampal volume with the entire cohort, age was a predictor of faster annual rate of hippocampal volume decline. None of the enrichment variables was associated with hippocampal volume.
DISCUSSION
The major conclusions of the study are as follows. (1) We observed minimal effects of lifestyle enrichment variables (education/occupation, midlife cognitive activity, and midlife physical activity) on the AD biomarker trajectories (specifically the rate of worsening). (2) Highly educated APOE4 carriers with high midlife cognitive activity have lower amyloid burden than highly educated APOE4 carriers with low midlife cognitive activity. (3) Age was associated with both amyloid and neurodegeneration biomarker trajectories; APOE4 status appears to influence only the amyloid and FDG trajectories but not hippocampal volume trajectory. (4) APOE4 status was not associated with lower FDG uptake in the high-education cohort but was associated with lower FDG uptake in the entire cohort and lower education cohort.
For all the models predicting amyloid deposition, older age and APOE4 status were significantly associated with higher levels of amyloid deposition. Only the entire cohort model showed an association between APOE4 status and faster rate of amyloid deposition, which can be attributed to the larger sample required for detecting small effects (observed coefficient was 0.01). These findings are consistent with the literature that has shown a significant impact of age and APOE4 on amyloid deposition.16,25,26 In our earlier cross-sectional studies, we did not find an association between amyloid and lifestyle enrichment21 similar to the results we found when we considered the entire cohort (figure 1). Some recent studies support the findings of no association between amyloid deposition and cognitive activity.3,–,5 However, after dichotomizing participants into high education and low education, we observed associations of high midlife cognitive activity with lower amyloid deposition in APOE4 participants with high education. The effects of lifestyle on amyloid deposition in our study are similar to those found in studies with participants with high education (median ≥16 years).6,–,10 There is also literature supporting our findings of reduced AD risk due to lifestyle modification, specifically in APOE4 carriers.10,27,–,30 The analyses conducted in this article make an important point that high-education cohorts are likely to find an association of high lifestyle enrichment with low amyloid deposition. The conclusions were the same when education and occupation were considered as separate variables.
There are several possible explanations for the observation that highly educated APOE4 carriers with high midlife cognitive activity have lower amyloid burden than highly educated APOE4 carriers with low midlife cognitive activity. (1) Higher levels of education and higher levels of midlife cognitive activities somehow forestall amyloid deposition, which is evident in APOE4 carriers because APOE4 carriers are accumulating amyloid at an earlier age and at a faster rate than APOE4 noncarriers.16,31 With greater power, we might see the same protective association in APOE4 noncarriers. The problem is that there is not a good biological explanation of how these features could forestall amyloid deposition. (2) A reverse causality explanation: APOE4 carriers are more likely to experience an earlier elevation of brain amyloid.16 Among all educated APOE4 carriers, those with the highest amyloid levels in middle age are most likely to experience subtle cognitive symptoms at that time and consequently avoid strenuous intellectual activity, thus placing them in the low cognitive activity group.
For all the models predicting FDG uptake, older age and APOE4 status were associated with lower glucose metabolism, which has been found in previous studies.32,–,34 An interesting finding was that APOE4 status was not associated with lower FDG uptake in the high-education subset, which implies that high education in APOE4 carriers may be protective via nonimpaired glucose metabolism. This may have been further observed by the differences in the amyloid deposition levels between participants with low cognitive activity vs high cognitive activity.
For the models predicting hippocampal volume, we found that older age was associated with lower hippocampal volume. Only the entire cohort model showed an association between older age and faster rate of hippocampal atrophy, which can be attributed to the larger sample required for detecting small effects (observed coefficient was −0.0025). APOE4 status did not influence the hippocampal atrophy trajectories. While decline of hippocampal volume with age has been well-established in the literature,35,36 the influence of APOE4 on hippocampal volume and rates of hippocampal atrophy has often been debated.37,38
In this longitudinal biomarker study, we did not find any direct effect of lifestyle enrichment variables on the rate of change of neurodegenerative biomarkers even though we had adequate power to detect associations of interest based on the sensitivity analyses. The effect of lifestyle enrichment on neurodegeneration is not unanimously agreed upon.3,4,8 The neurodegeneration biomarkers not only capture the disease-related effects (i.e., worsening of neurodegeneration with disease severity) but also capture structural and functional brain differences, e.g., genetic differences and compensatory mechanisms. Therefore, the effect of lifestyle enrichment on progression of neurodegeneration can yield different results depending on the biomarker and disease stage of the participants sampled.
There is substantial evidence that lifestyle enrichment is helpful in delaying the onset of cognitive impairment, but the mechanisms through which it delays impairment remain controversial. There may be 2 fundamental reasons for the controversial findings. First, the effect of lifestyle enrichment on AD biomarkers is small and only seen in select situations. Sample characteristics and sensitivity of the (lifestyle enrichment and biomarker) measures can yield significantly different findings. For example, in this study we did not see any effect of lifestyle enrichment in the overall sample, which is a population-based sample. Also, we only detected effects in highly educated individuals with APOE4 who had both higher levels of amyloid (APOE4 genotype) and higher neuroplasticity potential (high education). Second, there will be an impact of survivor and recruitment bias on the statistical models. For any biomedical study, persons with low educational attainment and low interest in novel activities tend to have a lower voluntary participate rate. Additionally, it is difficult to recruit older APOE4 carriers who do not have dementia due to the earlier onset of dementia in APOE4 carriers.
Unlike our prior work, we excluded data from current activities here because the existing neuropathologic processes may have an impact on the current physical and cognitive activities (i.e., reverse causality). We only included activity questionnaires that reported midlife physical and cognitive activities. The mean frequencies of the activities done by participants stratified by median education level (of 14 years) and median midlife cognitive activity level (of 19.5) are shown in table e-1 and figure e-2. Participants with high cognitive activity scores performed well across the board compared to those with low cognitive activity scores. The top 4 frequent activities in the high cognitive activity group were reading books and magazines, computer activities, and playing games. A third of the population sampled fell in the category of low education and low cognitive activity and would benefit from engaging in midlife cognitive activities.39 Though we did not find associations with midlife physical activities, ongoing and planned active interventional trials may be better designed to observe these associations with physical activity.40
The limitations of this study are as follows. (1) We took a simplistic approach of investigating the influence of lifestyle enrichment on the 3 AD biomarkers independently and did not consider the possible interactions between the different biomarker variables, which may be influenced by enrichment variables. (2) The AD biomarkers used in this study may not be capturing the brain compensatory mechanisms invoked by pathology due to lifestyle enrichment, e.g., recruitment of additional neuronal changes not measured here. (3) The activity questionnaires are self-reported.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Dr. Vemuri: study concept and design, study supervision, obtaining funding, interpretation of data, drafting and revising the manuscript for intellectual content. T.G. Lesnick: study design, analysis and interpretation of data. S.A. Pryzbelski: analysis and interpretation of data. Dr. Knopman: data collection, interpretation of data, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Machulda: data collection, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Lowe: data collection, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Mielke: data collection, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Roberts: data collection, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Gunter: analysis of data, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. M.L. Senjem: analysis of data, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Geda: data collection, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Rocca: data collection, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Petersen: obtained funding, data collection, revising the manuscript for intellectual content. Dr. Jack: obtaining funding, interpretation of data, data collection, revising the manuscript for intellectual content.
STUDY FUNDING
Supported by NIH grants R00 AG37573, P50 AG16574/P1, R01 AG11378, R01-AG041851, R01 AG034676, and U01 AG06786; and Opus building NIH grant C06 RR018898. The funding sources were not involved in the manuscript review or approval.
DISCLOSURE
P. Vemuri receives research funding from NIH/NIA. T. Lesnick and S. Przybelski report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. D. Knopman serves as Deputy Editor for Neurology®; serves on a Data Safety Monitoring Board for Lundbeck Pharmaceuticals and for the DIAN study; is an investigator in clinical trials sponsored by TauRX Pharmaceuticals, Lilly Pharmaceuticals, and the Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study; and receives research support from the NIH. M. Machulda receives research funding from the NIH. V. Lowe is a consultant for Bayer Schering Pharma and Piramal Imaging and receives research support from GE Healthcare, Siemens Molecular Imaging, AVID Radiopharmaceuticals, the NIH (NIA, NCI), the Elsie and Marvin Dekelboum Family Foundation, the MN Partnership for Biotechnology and Medical Genomics, and the Leukemia & Lymphoma Society. M. Mielke has served as a consultant for AbbVie and receives funding from NIH/NIA. R. Roberts receives research funding from the NIH. J. Gunter, M. Senjem, and Y. Geda report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. W. Rocca receives research funding from the NIH. R. Petersen reports receiving consulting fees from Elan Pharmaceuticals and GE Healthcare, receiving royalties from Oxford University Press, and serving as chair of data monitoring committees for Pfizer and Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy; and receives research support from the NIH/NIA. C. Jack serves as a consultant for Eli Lily and receives research funding from the NIH (R01-AG011378, RO1-AG041851, RO1-AG037551, U01-HL096917, U01-AG032438, U01-AG024904) and the Alexander Family Alzheimer's Disease Research Professorship of the Mayo Foundation. Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures.
Footnotes
Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article. The Article Processing Charge was paid by the authors.
Supplemental data at Neurology.org
- Received June 23, 2015.
- Accepted in final form November 25, 2015.
- © 2016 American Academy of Neurology
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND), which permits downloading and sharing the work provided it is properly cited. The work cannot be changed in any way or used commercially.
REFERENCES
- 1.↵
- 2.↵
- 3.↵
- 4.↵
- 5.↵
- Wilson RS,
- Boyle PA,
- Yu L,
- Barnes LL,
- Schneider JA,
- Bennett DA
- 6.↵
- 7.↵
- 8.↵
- 9.↵
- 10.↵
- Wirth M,
- Villeneuve S,
- La Joie R,
- Marks SM,
- Jagust WJ
- 11.↵
- 12.↵
- Petersen RC,
- Roberts RO,
- Knopman DS,
- et al
- 13.↵
- Roberts RO,
- Geda YE,
- Knopman DS,
- et al
- 14.↵
- 15.↵
- 16.↵
- 17.↵
- Lowe VJ,
- Kemp BJ,
- Jack CR Jr.,
- et al
- 18.↵
- 19.↵
- 20.↵
- Landau SM,
- Harvey D,
- Madison CM,
- et al
- 21.↵
- 22.↵
- 23.↵
- Ainsworth BE,
- Haskell WL,
- Herrmann SD,
- et al
- 24.↵
- Schelldorfer J,
- Buhlmann P,
- Van De Geer S
- 25.↵
- 26.↵
- 27.↵
- 28.↵
- 29.↵
- 30.↵
- 31.↵
- 32.↵
- 33.↵
- Kantarci K,
- Senjem ML,
- Lowe VJ,
- et al
- 34.↵
- Reiman EM,
- Caselli RJ,
- Chen K,
- Alexander GE,
- Bandy D,
- Frost J
- 35.↵
- Jack CR Jr.,
- Petersen RC,
- Xu Y,
- et al
- 36.↵
- Carlson NE,
- Moore MM,
- Dame A,
- et al
- 37.↵
- 38.↵
- 39.↵
- 40.↵
Letters: Rapid online correspondence
REQUIREMENTS
If you are uploading a letter concerning an article:
You must have updated your disclosures within six months: http://submit.neurology.org
Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.
If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.
Submission specifications:
- Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
- Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
- Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
- Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
- Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.
You May Also be Interested in
More Online
Dr. Jeffrey Allen and Dr. Nicholas Purcell
► Watch
Topics Discussed
Alert Me
Recommended articles
-
Article
Cognitive activity relates to cognitive performance but not to Alzheimer disease biomarkersChristopher M. Gidicsin, Jacqueline E. Maye, Joseph J. Locascio et al.Neurology, June 10, 2015 -
Articles
APOE modifies the association between Aβ load and cognition in cognitively normal older adultsK. Kantarci, V. Lowe, S.A. Przybelski et al.Neurology, December 21, 2011 -
Article
Weighting and standardization of frequencies to determine prevalence of AD imaging biomarkersRosebud O. Roberts, David S. Knopman, Jeremy A. Syrjanen et al.Neurology, October 13, 2017 -
Article
Association of education with Aβ burden in preclinical familial and sporadic Alzheimer diseaseJulie Gonneaud, Christophe Bedetti, Alexa Pichet Binette et al.Neurology, August 05, 2020