Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion (DEI)
    • Neurology: Clinical Practice Accelerator
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit New Manuscript
    • Submit Revised Manuscript
    • Author Center

Advanced Search

Main menu

  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion (DEI)
    • Neurology: Clinical Practice Accelerator
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit New Manuscript
    • Submit Revised Manuscript
    • Author Center
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Neurology
Home
The most widely read and highly cited peer-reviewed neurology journal
  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
Site Logo
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

Share

January 31, 2017; 88 (5) Article

Sleep characteristics and cognitive impairment in the general population

The HypnoLaus study

José Haba-Rubio, Helena Marti-Soler, Nadia Tobback, Daniela Andries, Pedro Marques-Vidal, Gérard Waeber, Peter Vollenweider, Armin von Gunten, Martin Preisig, Enrique Castelao, Mehdi Tafti, Raphaël Heinzer, Julius Popp
First published December 30, 2016, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003557
José Haba-Rubio
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Helena Marti-Soler
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nadia Tobback
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Daniela Andries
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Pedro Marques-Vidal
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gérard Waeber
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Peter Vollenweider
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Armin von Gunten
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Martin Preisig
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Enrique Castelao
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mehdi Tafti
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Raphaël Heinzer
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Julius Popp
From the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (J.H.-R., N.T., D.A., M.T., R.H.), Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine (H.M.-S), Department of Medicine, Service of Internal Medicine (P.M.-V., G.W., P.V.), Department of Psychiatry, Service of Old-Age Psychiatry (A.v.G., J.P.), Department of Psychiatry (M.P., E.C.), Center for Integrative Genomics (M.T.), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, and Leenaards Memory Center (J.P.), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and Lausanne University, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Full PDF
Citation
Sleep characteristics and cognitive impairment in the general population
The HypnoLaus study
José Haba-Rubio, Helena Marti-Soler, Nadia Tobback, Daniela Andries, Pedro Marques-Vidal, Gérard Waeber, Peter Vollenweider, Armin von Gunten, Martin Preisig, Enrique Castelao, Mehdi Tafti, Raphaël Heinzer, Julius Popp
Neurology Jan 2017, 88 (5) 463-469; DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003557

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Permissions

Make Comment

See Comments

Downloads
425

Share

  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Loading

Abstract

Objective: To assess the association between sleep structure and cognitive impairment in the general population.

Methods: Data stemmed from 580 participants aged >65 years of the population-based CoLaus/PsyCoLaus study (Lausanne, Switzerland) who underwent complete sleep evaluation (HypnoLaus). Evaluations included demographic characteristics, personal and treatment history, sleep complaints and habits (using validated questionnaires), and a complete polysomnography at home. Cognitive function was evaluated using a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery and a questionnaire on the participant's everyday activities. Participants with cognitive impairment (global Clinical Dementia Rating [CDR] scale score > 0) were compared with participants with no cognitive impairment (global CDR score = 0).

Results: The 291 participants with a CDR score > 0 (72.5 ± 4.6 years), compared to the 289 controls with CDR = 0 (72.1 ± 4.6 years), had significantly more light (stage N1) and less deep (stage N3) and REM sleep, as well as lower sleep efficiency, higher intrasleep wake, and higher sleepiness scores (all p < 0.05). Sleep-disordered breathing was more severe in participants with cognitive impairment with an apnea/hypopnea index (AHI) of 18.0 (7.8–35.5)/h (p50 [p25–p75]) (vs 12.9 [7.2–24.5]/h, p < 0.001), and higher oxygen desaturation index (ODI). In the multivariate analysis after adjustments for confounding variables, the AHI and the ODI ≥4% and ≥6% were independently associated with cognitive impairment.

Conclusions: Participants aged >65 years with cognitive impairment have higher sleepiness scores and a more disrupted sleep. This seems to be related to the occurrence of sleep-disordered breathing and the associated intermittent hypoxia.

GLOSSARY

AASM=
American Academy of Sleep Medicine;
AD=
Alzheimer dementia;
AHI=
apnea-hypopnea index;
BMI=
body mass index;
CDR=
Clinical Dementia Rating;
CI=
cognitive impairment;
DSM-IV=
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition;
ESS=
Epworth Sleepiness Scale;
MCI=
mild cognitive impairment;
MDD=
major depressive disorder;
MEQ=
Horne and Östberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire;
ODI=
oxygen desaturation index;
PLMS=
periodic leg movements during sleep;
PSG=
polysomnography;
PSQI=
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index;
SDB=
sleep-disordered breathing;
TST=
total sleep time;
WHR=
waist-hip ratio

Sleep is a vital biological function, essential for brain restoration and memory consolidation.1 Significant changes in sleep quantity and architecture occur through life,2 with increased susceptibility to sleep disorders with aging.3 Sleep disturbances are particularly frequent in individuals with cognitive deficits such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementias.4 These often have been dismissed as consequences of the disease process underlying the cognitive decline, or of its related somatic and psychiatric comorbidities. Nevertheless, accumulating evidence suggests that sleep duration,5,–,8 sleep fragmentation,9,–,12 and sleep pathologies, such as sleep-disordered breathing (SDB),13,14 can play a role in the pathogenic process leading to cognitive impairment (CI). These studies suggest that alterations in sleep might be an early marker or precipitating factors of cognitive decline, but most relied on self-reported sleep duration or rest-activity cycles measured by actigraphy.

Polysomnography (PSG) is the gold standard to measure sleep, giving detailed information about sleep stages and disorders, and can provide a deeper insight into the relationship between CI and sleep, but only a limited number of studies have used PSG to assess sleep in this context.13,–,15 In addition, some previous studies have been conducted in clinical or selected populations (i.e., only women13 or only men14,15), did not provide data on both subjective and objective sleep characteristics, and many of them did not control for conditions known to impair sleep and cognition (e.g., cardiovascular disorders, medications, depression).

Using a large and well-characterized sample drawn from the general population, the aim of this epidemiologic observational study was to compare subjective sleep characteristics, assessed by questionnaires, and objective sleep features, measured by PSG, of elderly participants with CI to those of participants with normal cognition.

METHODS

Population sampling.

Data stemmed from participants of the population-based CoLaus/PsyCoLaus study.16,17 Briefly, the baseline CoLaus/PsyCoLaus study (between 2003 and 2006) included a representative sample of 6,733 participants (aged 35–75 years) selected from the residents of the city of Lausanne, Switzerland. The age, sex distribution, and zip codes of the participants were comparable to the source population.16 Five years after the baseline investigation, participants underwent a new physical (n = 5,064) and psychiatric (n = 4,002) evaluation.

Cognitive evaluation.

During the psychiatric follow-up investigation, 1,128 participants older than 65 years accepted a cognitive evaluation (72.7% participation rate). The neuropsychological test battery, performed by trained neuropsychologists, included the Mini-Mental State Examination,18 the Grober and Buschke Double Memory Test,19 the DO 80 naming task,20 the Stroop Test,21 the letter fluency task,22 and the figures from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease neuropsychological test battery.23 Overall cognitive and functional status was determined using the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scale, a validated scale used for the clinical staging of CI that incorporates information concerning cognitive performance and related activities of daily life in 6 areas: memory, orientation, judgment and problem solving, community affairs, home and hobbies, and personal care.24 The participants with a global CDR score = 0 were considered to have normal cognitive abilities while the participants with CDR > 0 were considered to have CI.24

Sleep evaluation.

As a nested project of CoLaus/PsyCoLaus, the HypnoLaus study evaluated sleep characteristics of this population (figure e-1 at Neurology.org). We evaluated subjective sleep habits and complaints with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI),25 the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS),26 the Horne and Östberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ),27 and the Berlin Questionnaire for sleep apnea risk.28

In addition, we invited 3,043 consecutive participants to undergo a full-night PSG recording at home, and 2,162 accepted (71% participation rate; 723 older than 65 years). Participants were equipped with the PSG recorder (Titanium, Embla Flaga, Reykjavik, Iceland) by qualified technicians between 5 and 8 pm at the Center for Investigation and Research in Sleep (Lausanne University Hospital, Switzerland), and sleep recordings took place in the participants' home environment. The participants were not selected on the basis of the responses to questionnaires and the investigators were unaware of the questionnaires' results. Participants under continuous positive airway pressure for SDB (n = 38, 1.75% of the entire sample) were asked to stop their treatment 1 week prior to the PSG.

We used the same methodology for the PSG recordings as the one described in previous studies.3,29 PSG recordings included the following channels: 4 EEG (F3/M2, F4/M1, C3/M2, C4/M1, O1/M2, and O2/M1), electro-oculography (right and left), surface EMG channels (submental and right and left anterior tibialis), ECG, nasal pressure, thoracic and abdominal respiratory inductance belts, body position, oxygen saturation, and pulse rate in accordance with the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) 2007 recommended setup.30

PSG recordings were visually scored by 2 trained sleep technicians using Somnologica software (version 5.1.1, Embla Flaga) and reviewed by an expert sleep physician. A second sleep expert performed random quality checks. Sleep stages, arousals, and respiratory events were scored according to the 2007 AASM recommended criteria.30 Periodic leg movements during sleep (PLMS) were scored according to the official World Association of Sleep Medicine standards.31 Apnea was defined as a drop of at least 90% of airflow from baseline lasting 10 seconds or longer. Hypopnea was defined as a ≥30% drop of airflow for ≥10 seconds associated with a ≥4% O2 drop. The average number of apneas/hypopneas per hour of sleep (apnea-hypopnea index [AHI]) was calculated. The oxygen desaturation index (ODI) represents the number of oxygen saturation drops per hour of sleep (≥3%, ≥4%, or ≥6%). The periodic leg movement in sleep index was calculated by dividing the total number of PLMS by total sleep time (TST) in hours.

Other variables.

Information on sociodemographic characteristics, medical and treatment history, smoking habits (current smoker/ex-smoker or never smoker), and alcohol consumption (currently drinking or no alcohol consumption) was elicited by trained interviewers using questionnaires. Educational level was characterized as primary school, high (secondary) school, professional school, and university or non-university higher education. Medication at the time of the sleep evaluation was coded according to the WHO ATC classification (whocc.no/atcddd). We considered hypnotics (ATC code: N05CF), benzodiazepines or derivates (N05BA, N05CD, N03AE), antidepressants (N06A), neuroleptics (N05A), and antihistaminics (R06A) as the drug categories having a potential major effect on sleep.

During the physical examination, we collected fasting blood samples for various clinical analyses (including glucose, cholesterol, and triglycerides). We measured the height and weight and we calculated the body mass index (BMI) and the waist-hip ratio (WHR). We measured blood pressure in triplicate and averaged values between the last 2 readings. We defined arterial hypertension as a systolic blood pressure ≥140 mm Hg or a diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mm Hg or if the participant was taking antihypertensive drugs. We defined diabetes as a fasting blood glucose level ≥7 mmol/L (126 mg/dL) or if the participant was taking antidiabetic drugs (oral hypoglycemic or insulin). We defined the metabolic syndrome according to the Adult Treatment Panel III report.

We collected diagnostic information on mental disorders according to the DSM-IV, including major depressive disorder (MDD), during the psychiatric evaluation using the French version of the semi-structured Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies.32

Statistical analysis.

We performed all statistical analysis with Stata 11 (StataCorp, College Station, TX). For descriptive statistics, we summarized continuous variables as mean ± SD and categorical variables as number of participants or percentages. We also used descriptive statistics to show sleep characteristics of the participants based on presence or absence of cognitive deficits. We used Student t tests, χ2 tests, or one-way analysis of variance to assess univariate differences between sleep variables, one at a time.

At a second step, to determine the independent association of PSG-derived variables with a CDR > 0, we constructed a multivariate logistic regression model for each PSG variable after adjustment for age, sex, hypertension, diabetes, metabolic syndrome, current MDD, lifetime MDD, BMI, alcohol and tobacco consumption, drugs influencing sleep, and level of education. We expressed results as multivariate-adjusted odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for 10-unit increase of each sleep-related variable. We considered statistical significance for a 2-sided test p < 0.05.

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents.

The Ethics Committee of the University of Lausanne approved CoLaus/PsyCoLaus and HypnoLaus studies, and all participants signed a written informed consent.

RESULTS

Characteristics of the sample.

Of 1,552 participants aged >65 years at the first follow-up of CoLaus/PsyCoLaus, 580 (37.4%) completed both the neuropsychological evaluation and a PSG, and constitute the sample for the present analyses. These participants were slightly younger (70.4 ± 4.4 vs 71.0 ± 4.9, p = 0.017), but did not differ regarding other characteristics from the other participants older than 65 years (table 1).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1

General characteristics of the CoLaus/PsyCoLaus population >65 years and studied population (participants with Clinical Dementia Rating [CDR] evaluation and polysomnography)

Comparison of participants with and without CI.

CI (CDR > 0) was present in 291 participants, representing 50.2% of the sample. All included participants with CI had a CDR = 0.5, which is generally considered to correspond to mild CI. The results of the comparison between participants with and without CI are summarized in table 2. Participants with CI included a higher proportion of men, regular alcohol consumption, a higher WHR but a similar BMI, and a higher incidence of hypertension and diabetes. No significant differences were found for age, prevalence of metabolic syndrome, or current or lifetime MDD. Results of the neuropsychological tests according to CDR status are presented in table e-1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2

General characteristics of the studied population (participants with Clinical Dementia Rating [CDR] evaluation and polysomnography) according to CDR status

Subjective sleep characteristics.

Table 3 summarizes the subjective sleep characteristics of the sample according to CDR status. Compared to participants with no CI, participants with a CDR > 0 had higher ESS scores, yet still in the normal ranges (5.4 ± 3.2 vs 4.7 ± 3.4, p = 0.024), and the percentage of participants presenting excessive daytime sleepiness (ESS > 10) was similar in both groups.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3

Subjective sleep characteristics according to Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) status

More participants with CI exhibited sleep apnea risk, according to the Berlin questionnaire (34.0% vs 25.1%, p = 0.019). There were no significant differences in global sleep quality, with similar mean PSQI score and similar percentages of PSQI >5, or concerning subjective sleep latency or subjective sleep duration. Finally, no significant differences emerged concerning their chronotype, with similar mean scores at the MEQ.

Objective sleep characteristics.

The results of objective sleep characteristics according to presence or absence of CI are summarized in figure 1 and in table e-2. Participants with CI spent more sleep time in light (stage N1) sleep and less time in deep (stage N3) sleep and in REM sleep than participants without CI. Sleep efficiency of participants with CI was also significantly lower, with higher intrasleep wake. Participants with CI had more severe SDB, with a higher mean AHI. Even though the mean SaO2 during sleep was similar in both groups, the SaO2 nadir was lower in participants with CI, as well as the oxygen desaturation indexes, using different oxygen desaturation thresholds. No differences were found concerning other sleep-related parameters.

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1 Objective sleep characteristics according to Clinical Dementia Rating scale (CDR) status

*p < 0.05. AHI = apnea/hypopnea index (n/h); LowestSaO2 = lowest oxygen saturation (in %) during sleep; MeanSaO2 = mean oxygen saturation (in %) during sleep; ODI3 = oxygen desaturation index ≥3% during sleep (n/h); ODI4 = oxygen desaturation index ≥4% during sleep (n/h); ODI6 = oxygen desaturation index ≥6% during sleep (n/h); REM = REM sleep (in % of total sleep time); REMlat = latency to REM sleep from sleep onset (min); St1 = stage N1 sleep (in % of total sleep time); SE = sleep efficiency; SOL = sleep onset latency (min); St2 = stage N2 sleep (in % of total sleep time); St3 = stage N3 sleep (in % of total sleep time); TST = total sleep time (min); WASO = wake after sleep onset (min).

Multivariate analysis of the association between objective sleep characteristics and CI.

For PSG-derived variables, we constructed a multivariable model, to determine independent associations. For each variable the model was adjusted for age, sex, BMI, alcohol and tobacco consumption, psychotropic drugs influencing sleep, level of education, and the presence of hypertension, diabetes, metabolic syndrome, and MDD (figure 2 and table e-3).

Figure 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2 Association of polysomnographic variables with a Clinical Dementia Rating scale score >0, multivariate analysis

Odds ratio for an increase of 10 units. Multivariate logistic regression model adjusted for each variable for age, sex, hypertension, diabetes, metabolic syndrome, current depression, lifetime depression, body mass index, alcohol and tobacco consumption, drugs influencing sleep, and level of education. AHI = apnea/hypopnea index (n/h); ArI = arousal index (n/h); ODI3 = oxygen desaturation index ≥3% during sleep (n/h); ODI4 = oxygen desaturation index ≥4% during sleep (n/h); ODI6 = oxygen desaturation index ≥6% during sleep (n/h); PLMSI = periodic leg movement in sleep index (n/h); REM = REM sleep (in % of total sleep time); REMlat = latency to REM sleep from sleep onset (min); St1 = stage N1 sleep (in % of total sleep time); SE = sleep efficiency; SOL = sleep onset latency (min); St2 = stage N2 sleep (in % of total sleep time); St3 = stage N3 sleep (in % of total sleep time); StShifts = number of stage shifts; TST = total sleep time (min); WASO = wake after sleep onset (min).

In this model only AHI, oxygen desaturation index ≥4%, and oxygen desaturation index ≥6% remained significantly and independently associated with the presence of CI. In more detailed analysis with increasing sleep apnea severity, measured by the AHI, we observed a worsening of specific cognitive scores, in particular the Grober and Buschke total free recall (ρ = −0.126, p = 0.004) and delayed free recall (ρ = −0.092, p = 0.038) scores, semantic verbal fluency (ρ = −0.105, p = 0.017), and Stroop dots condition (ρ = −0.121, p = 0.004).

DISCUSSION

We analyzed the sleep characteristics of participants with mild CI in a large sample of participants >65 years from the general population. Compared to participants with no CI, they presented significant differences in sleep patterns with more time spent in stage N1 and less in stage N3 and in REM sleep, lower sleep efficiency, and more wake after sleep onset according to univariate analyses. They also exhibited more severe SDB, with higher AHI and ODI, and higher ESS scores. However, multivariate analysis after adjustment for possible confounding factors revealed that only SDB indices (AHI, ODI ≥4%, and ODI ≥6%) were independently associated with impaired performance on cognitive tests.

Previous epidemiologic studies from general populations have suggested that sleep quantity is associated with cognition: self-reported short sleep or long sleep are associated with poorer cognitive function5,7 and with a higher MCI/dementia risk.6 It has also been reported that subjective sleep quality or having a complaint of excessive sleepiness during the day are associated with decreased cognitive performance and a higher risk of cognitive decline and dementia.5,33 In some studies, this association seems mediated by other variables, such as comorbidities.34 Prospective studies have shown that increased sleep fragmentation,12 longer sleep latency, and lower sleep efficiency and TST,8 measured using actigraphy, are associated with cognitive decline and incident Alzheimer dementia (AD), and that better sleep consolidation attenuates the effect of the ε4 allele of APOE on cognitive decline, the risk of AD, and cerebral neurofibrillary tangle density.9 It has also been shown that decreased sleep efficiency measured by actigraphy, as well as reported frequent naps, were associated with higher levels of cerebral amyloid deposition as assessed by CSF Aβ42 levels,10 and that among community-dwelling older adults, reports of shorter sleep duration and poorer sleep quality were associated with greater Aβ burden, assessed by Pittsburgh compound B PET imaging.11

In our study, we also found some of the previously described sleep alterations, as participants with CI had a more fragmented sleep, with more light sleep and less deep sleep and REM sleep, similar to that found in the prospective Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Study,15 and higher sleepiness scores. However, in the multivariate analysis and after accounting for potential confounders, only SDB measures remained significantly associated with cognitive deficits. SDB can be the cause of changes in sleep structure found here, as it provokes sleep fragmentation, profound changes in sleep architecture, and sleepiness.

Cross-sectional studies of SDB and cognitive function in elderly populations have reported conflicting results,35,–,38 which may be due to population selection, differences in the measurement and definition of SDB, or the type of cognitive evaluation used. In a prospective substudy of the Study of Osteoporotic Fractures, the analysis of data from 298 women (mean age 82.3 years) showed that a higher proportion of those with prevalent SDB (AHI >15/h) at the initial evaluation developed mild CI or dementia in the 5 years of follow-up.13 This study implies that SDB precedes dementia.

Each of the 2 main characteristics of SDB—sleep fragmentation and hypoxia—could have negative effects on cognitive function. In the Study of Osteoporotic Fractures, 2 measures of hypoxia (an ODI ≥3% of ≥15/h and a high percentage of TST in apnea or hypopnea) were associated with higher incidence of CI. Nocturnal hypoxemia and ODI were also predictors of global cognitive decline in the 2,636 community-dwelling older men (age 76.0 ± 5.3 years) without CI followed for 3.4 ± 0.5 years in the Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Sleep Study.14 Similarly, in our study, ODI (≥4% and ≥6%) was independently associated with the presence of CI. Conversely, no independent significant association was found for sleep duration or measures of sleep fragmentation, such as the arousal index or the intrasleep wake. This suggests that hypoxia is likely to be the mechanism through which SDB is associated with CI. Furthermore, the fact that the mean SaO2 during sleep time was not significantly associated with CI suggests that intermittent hypoxia, rather than continuous hypoxia, is the mechanism behind this relationship. It has been shown that intermittent hypoxia is associated with increased risk of oxidative stress and adverse outcomes.39

These findings have major clinical implications given that SDB is a common3 and treatable condition. Indeed, studies suggest that SDB treatment may partially restore CI and slow cognitive deterioration.40

The major strengths of our study are its population-based design, the large sample size for this type of studies, the access of comprehensive data on a large number of sociodemographic and clinical variables, the consideration and statistical control of significant potential confounders, the use of PSG to objectively assess sleep duration, sleep architecture, and sleep-related comorbidities, and the inclusion of subjective sleep evaluations, as both objective and subjective measures are important and complementary to characterize sleep.

Our study has several potential limitations. The cross-sectional design of the study does not allow drawing conclusions on the directionality of the association between sleep disturbances and CI. Moreover, the proportion of participants who underwent both cognitive testing and PSG was low, as only 37.4% of the potential candidates had both cognitive and sleep tests. The absence of statistical significance for certain associations could be due to the sample size. Furthermore, the underlying cerebral pathologies in the participants with CI remain unknown. Participants in this study were almost exclusively of European origin. Therefore, our findings will not necessarily generalize to ethnically different populations.

We found that participants in this study from the general population aged >65 years with CI, compared to participants with no cognitive deficits, have a more disrupted sleep. This was associated with the occurrence of SDB, SDB indices being the only ones to be independently associated with an increased risk of CI. Our results suggest that this relationship is related to the severity of the SDB-induced intermittent hypoxia. Additional longitudinal and interventional studies are needed to confirm these results.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

J.H.-R., R.H., and J.P. were responsible for the study concept, design, data interpretation, and writing of the manuscript. N.T. and D.A. acquired and analyzed the polysomnographic data. H.M.-S. performed the statistical analysis. P.M.-V., G.W., P.V., A.v.G., M.P., E.C., and M.T. were responsible for the CoLaus/PsyCoLaus and HypnoLaus studies concept and design, supervised the study. and revised the manuscript for intellectual content.

STUDY FUNDING

The CoLaus/PsyCoLaus study is supported by research grants from GlaxoSmithKline, the Faculty of Biology and Medicine of Lausanne, and the Swiss National Science Foundation (grants 3200B0-105993, 3200B0-118308, 33CSCO-122661, 33CS30-139468, and 33CS30-148401). The HypnoLaus study received additional support from the Leenaards Foundation and Vaud Pulmonary League (Ligue Pulmonaire Vaudoise). J.P. was supported by a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (320030L-141179).

DISCLOSURE

J. Haba-Rubio, H. Marti-Soler, N. Tobback, D. Andries, P. Marques-Vidal, G. Waeber, P. Vollenweider, A. von Gunten, M. Preisig, E. Castelao, and M. Tafti report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. R. Heinzer is part of the Advisory Board of NightBalance Company. J. Popp reports no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The authors thank Prof. Vincent Mooser, the investigators, and the participants of the CoLaus/PsyCoLaus and HypnoLaus cohorts.

Footnotes

  • ↵* These authors contributed equally to this work.

  • Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article.

  • Editorial, page 424

  • Supplemental data at Neurology.org

  • Received April 6, 2016.
  • Accepted in final form October 3, 2016.
  • © 2016 American Academy of Neurology

REFERENCES

  1. 1.↵
    1. Diekelmann S,
    2. Born J
    . The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010;11:114–126.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. 2.↵
    1. Luca G,
    2. Haba Rubio J,
    3. Andries D, et al
    . Age and gender variations of sleep in subjects without sleep disorders. Ann Med 2015;47:482–491.
    OpenUrl
  3. 3.↵
    1. Heinzer R,
    2. Vat S,
    3. Marques-Vidal P, et al
    . Prevalence of sleep-disordered breathing in the general population: the HypnoLaus study. Lancet Respir Med 2015;3:310–318.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. 4.↵
    1. Moran M,
    2. Lynch CA,
    3. Walsh C,
    4. Coen R,
    5. Coakley D,
    6. Lawlor BA
    . Sleep disturbance in mild to moderate Alzheimer's disease. Sleep Med 2005;6:347–352.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. 5.↵
    1. Potvin O,
    2. Lorrain D,
    3. Forget H, et al
    . Sleep quality and 1-year incident cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older adults. Sleep 2012;35:491–499.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. 6.↵
    1. Chen JC,
    2. Espeland MA,
    3. Brunner RL, et al
    . Sleep duration, cognitive decline, and dementia risk in older women. Alzheimer's Dement 2016;12:21–33.
    OpenUrl
  7. 7.↵
    1. Kronholm E,
    2. Sallinen M,
    3. Suutama T,
    4. Sulkava R,
    5. Era P,
    6. Partonen T
    . Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive functioning in the general population. J Sleep Res 2009;18:436–446.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Diem SJ,
    2. Blackwell TL,
    3. Stone KL, et al
    . Measures of sleep-wake patterns and risk of mild cognitive impairment or dementia in older women. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016;24:248–258.
    OpenUrl
  9. 9.↵
    1. Lim AS,
    2. Yu L,
    3. Kowgier M,
    4. Schneider JA,
    5. Buchman AS,
    6. Bennett DA
    . Modification of the relationship of the apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele to the risk of Alzheimer disease and neurofibrillary tangle density by sleep. JAMA Neurol 2013;70:1544–1551.
    OpenUrl
  10. 10.↵
    1. Ju YE,
    2. McLeland JS,
    3. Toedebusch CD, et al
    . Sleep quality and preclinical Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol 2013;70:587–593.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. 11.↵
    1. Spira AP,
    2. Gamaldo AA,
    3. An Y, et al
    . Self-reported sleep and beta-amyloid deposition in community-dwelling older adults. JAMA Neurol 2013;70:1537–1543.
    OpenUrl
  12. 12.↵
    1. Lim AS,
    2. Kowgier M,
    3. Yu L,
    4. Buchman AS,
    5. Bennett DA
    . Sleep fragmentation and the risk of incident Alzheimer's disease and cognitive decline in older persons. Sleep 2013;36:1027–1032.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. 13.↵
    1. Yaffe K,
    2. Laffan AM,
    3. Harrison SL, et al
    . Sleep-disordered breathing, hypoxia, and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older women. JAMA 2011;306:613–619.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. 14.↵
    1. Blackwell T,
    2. Yaffe K,
    3. Laffan A, et al
    . Associations between sleep-disordered breathing, nocturnal hypoxemia, and subsequent cognitive decline in older community-dwelling men: the Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Sleep Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2015;63:453–461.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. 15.↵
    1. Song Y,
    2. Blackwell T,
    3. Yaffe K, et al
    . Relationships between sleep stages and changes in cognitive function in older men: the MrOS Sleep Study. Sleep 2015;38:411–421.
    OpenUrl
  16. 16.↵
    1. Firmann M,
    2. Mayor V,
    3. Vidal PM, et al
    . The CoLaus study: a population-based study to investigate the epidemiology and genetic determinants of cardiovascular risk factors and metabolic syndrome. BMC Cardiovasc Disord 2008;8:6.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. 17.↵
    1. Preisig M,
    2. Waeber G,
    3. Vollenweider P, et al
    . The PsyCoLaus study: methodology and characteristics of the sample of a population-based survey on psychiatric disorders and their association with genetic and cardiovascular risk factors. BMC Psychiatry 2009;9:9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    1. Folstein MF,
    2. Folstein SE,
    3. McHugh PR
    . “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–198.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. 19.↵
    1. Buschke H,
    2. Sliwinski MJ,
    3. Kuslansky G,
    4. Lipton RB
    . Diagnosis of early dementia by the Double Memory Test: encoding specificity improves diagnostic sensitivity and specificity. Neurology 1997;48:989–997.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. 20.↵
    1. Deloche G,
    2. Hannequin D
    . DO 80: Epreuve de Dénomination Orale d'Images. Paris: Paris Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée; 1997.
  21. 21.↵
    1. Stroop JR
    . Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935;18:643–662.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. 22.↵
    1. Cardebat D,
    2. Doyon B,
    3. Puel M,
    4. Goulet P,
    5. Joanette Y
    . Formal and semantic lexical evocation in normal subjects: performance and dynamics of production as a function of sex, age and educational level. Acta Neurol Bel 1990;90:207–217.
    OpenUrl
  23. 23.↵
    1. Morris JC,
    2. Mohs RC,
    3. Rogers H,
    4. Fillenbaum G,
    5. Heyman A
    . Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Psychopharmacol Bull 1988;24:641–652.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. 24.↵
    1. Morris JC
    . The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993;43:2412–2414.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  25. 25.↵
    1. Buysse DJ,
    2. Reynolds CF III.,
    3. Monk TH,
    4. Berman SR,
    5. Kupfer DJ
    . The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989;28:193–213.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. 26.↵
    1. Johns MW
    . A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep 1991;14:540–545.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  27. 27.↵
    1. Horne JA,
    2. Ostberg O
    . A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 1976;4:97–110.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. 28.↵
    1. Netzer NC,
    2. Stoohs RA,
    3. Netzer CM,
    4. Clark K,
    5. Strohl KP
    . Using the Berlin Questionnaire to identify patients at risk for the sleep apnea syndrome. Ann Intern Med 1999;131:485–491.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. 29.↵
    1. Haba-Rubio J,
    2. Marti-Soler H,
    3. Marques-Vidal P, et al
    . Prevalence and determinants of periodic limb movements in the general population. Ann Neurol 2016;79:464–474.
    OpenUrl
  30. 30.↵
    American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. 1st ed. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2007.
  31. 31.↵
    1. Zucconi M,
    2. Ferri R,
    3. Allen R, et al
    . The official World Association of Sleep Medicine (WASM) standards for recording and scoring periodic leg movements in sleep (PLMS) and wakefulness (PLMW) developed in collaboration with a task force from the International Restless Legs Syndrome Study Group (IRLSSG). Sleep Med 2006;7:175–183.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. 32.↵
    1. Preisig M,
    2. Fenton BT,
    3. Matthey ML,
    4. Berney A,
    5. Ferrero F
    . Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (DIGS): inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the French version. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;249:174–179.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. 33.↵
    1. Jaussent I,
    2. Bouyer J,
    3. Ancelin ML, et al
    . Excessive sleepiness is predictive of cognitive decline in the elderly. Sleep 2012;35:1201–1207.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  34. 34.↵
    1. Jelicic M,
    2. Bosma H,
    3. Ponds RW,
    4. Van Boxtel MP,
    5. Houx PJ,
    6. Jolles J
    . Subjective sleep problems in later life as predictors of cognitive decline: report from the Maastricht Ageing Study (MAAS). Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002;17:73–77.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. 35.↵
    1. Spira AP,
    2. Blackwell T,
    3. Stone KL, et al
    . Sleep-disordered breathing and cognition in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc 2008;56:45–50.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. 36.↵
    1. Foley DJ,
    2. Masaki K,
    3. White L,
    4. Larkin EK,
    5. Monjan A,
    6. Redline S
    . Sleep-disordered breathing and cognitive impairment in elderly Japanese-American men. Sleep 2003;26:596–599.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  37. 37.↵
    1. Sforza E,
    2. Roche F,
    3. Thomas-Anterion C, et al
    . Cognitive function and sleep related breathing disorders in a healthy elderly population: the SYNAPSE study. Sleep 2010;33:515–521.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. 38.↵
    1. Ramos AR,
    2. Tarraf W,
    3. Rundek T, et al
    . Obstructive sleep apnea and neurocognitive function in a Hispanic/Latino population. Neurology 2015;84:391–398.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. 39.↵
    1. Arnaud C,
    2. Dematteis M,
    3. Pepin JL,
    4. Baguet JP,
    5. Levy P
    . Obstructive sleep apnea, immuno-inflammation, and atherosclerosis. Semin Immunopathol 2009;31:113–125.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. 40.↵
    1. Cooke JR,
    2. Ayalon L,
    3. Palmer BW, et al
    . Sustained use of CPAP slows deterioration of cognition, sleep, and mood in patients with Alzheimer's disease and obstructive sleep apnea: a preliminary study. J Clin Sleep Med 2009;5:305–309.
    OpenUrlPubMed

Letters: Rapid online correspondence

No comments have been published for this article.
Comment

REQUIREMENTS

You must ensure that your Disclosures have been updated within the previous six months. Please go to our Submission Site to add or update your Disclosure information.

Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.

If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.

Submission specifications:

  • Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
  • Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
  • Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
  • Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
  • Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.

More guidelines and information on Disputes & Debates

Compose Comment

More information about text formats

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
Author Information
NOTE: The first author must also be the corresponding author of the comment.
First or given name, e.g. 'Peter'.
Your last, or family, name, e.g. 'MacMoody'.
Your email address, e.g. higgs-boson@gmail.com
Your role and/or occupation, e.g. 'Orthopedic Surgeon'.
Your organization or institution (if applicable), e.g. 'Royal Free Hospital'.
Publishing Agreement
NOTE: All authors, besides the first/corresponding author, must complete a separate Publishing Agreement Form and provide via email to the editorial office before comments can be posted.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.

Vertical Tabs

You May Also be Interested in

Back to top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • GLOSSARY
    • METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
    • STUDY FUNDING
    • DISCLOSURE
    • ACKNOWLEDGMENT
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Advertisement

Safety and Efficacy of Tenecteplase and Alteplase in Patients With Tandem Lesion Stroke: A Post Hoc Analysis of the EXTEND-IA TNK Trials

Dr. Nicole Sur and Dr. Mausaminben Hathidara

► Watch

Related Articles

  • The link between sleep-disordered breathing and cognition in the elderlyNew opportunities?

Topics Discussed

  • All Sleep Disorders
  • Sleep apnea
  • MCI (mild cognitive impairment)

Alert Me

  • Alert me when eletters are published

Recommended articles

  • Views and Reviews
    Role of sleep-disordered breathing and sleep-wake disturbances for stroke and stroke recovery
    Dirk M. Hermann, Claudio L. Bassetti et al.
    Neurology, August 03, 2016
  • Article
    Sleep-disordered breathing among patients admitted for inpatient video-EEG monitoring
    Shobi Sivathamboo, Sarah Farrand, Zhibin Chen et al.
    Neurology, December 14, 2018
  • Article
    Objective daytime sleepiness in patients with somnambulism or sleep terrors
    Régis Lopez, Isabelle Jaussent, Yves Dauvilliers et al.
    Neurology, October 29, 2014
  • Article
    Periodic limb movements during sleep in stroke/TIA
    Prevalence, course, and cardiovascular burden
    Mauro Manconi, Francesco Fanfulla, Raffaele Ferri et al.
    Neurology, April 11, 2018
Neurology: 100 (22)

Articles

  • Ahead of Print
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Popular Articles
  • Translations

About

  • About the Journals
  • Ethics Policies
  • Editors & Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertise

Submit

  • Author Center
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Information for Reviewers
  • AAN Guidelines
  • Permissions

Subscribers

  • Subscribe
  • Activate a Subscription
  • Sign up for eAlerts
  • RSS Feed
Site Logo
  • Visit neurology Template on Facebook
  • Follow neurology Template on Twitter
  • Visit Neurology on YouTube
  • Neurology
  • Neurology: Clinical Practice
  • Neurology: Education
  • Neurology: Genetics
  • Neurology: Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • AAN.com
  • AANnews
  • Continuum
  • Brain & Life
  • Neurology Today

Wolters Kluwer Logo

Neurology | Print ISSN:0028-3878
Online ISSN:1526-632X

© 2023 American Academy of Neurology

  • Privacy Policy
  • Feedback
  • Advertise