Effect of Cognitive Reserve on the Association of Vascular Brain Injury With Cognition
Analysis of the PURE and CAHHM Studies
Citation Manager Formats
Make Comment
See Comments

Abstract
Background and Objectives To determine whether cognitive reserve attenuates the association of vascular brain injury with cognition.
Methods Cross-sectional data were analyzed from 2 harmonized studies: the Canadian Alliance for Healthy Hearts and Healthy Minds (CAHHM) and the Prospective Urban and Rural Epidemiology (PURE) study. Markers of cognitive reserve were education, involvement in social activities, marital status, height, and leisure physical activity, which were combined into a composite score. Vascular brain injury was defined as nonlacunar brain infarcts or high white matter hyperintensity (WMH) burden on MRI. Cognition was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment Tool (MoCA) and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST).
Results There were 10,916 participants age 35–81. Mean age was 58.8 years (range 35–81) and 55.8% were female. Education, moderate leisure physical activity, being in a marital partnership, being taller, and participating in social groups were each independently associated with higher cognition, as was the composite cognitive reserve score. Vascular brain injury was associated with lower cognition (β –0.35 [95% confidence interval [CI] –0.53 to −0.17] for MoCA and β −2.19 [95% CI −3.22 to −1.15] for DSST) but the association was not modified by the composite cognitive reserve variable (interaction p = 0.59 for MoCA and p = 0.72 for DSST).
Conclusions Both vascular brain injury and markers of cognitive reserve are associated with cognition. However, the effects were independent such that the adverse effects of covert vascular brain injury were not attenuated by higher cognitive reserve. To improve cognitive brain health, interventions to both prevent cerebrovascular disease and promote positive lifestyles are needed.
Glossary
- CAHHM=
- Canadian Alliance for Healthy Hearts and Healthy Minds;
- CBI=
- covert brain infarct;
- CI=
- confidence interval;
- DSST=
- Digit Symbol Substitution;
- FLAIR=
- fluid-attenuated inversion recovery;
- LADIS=
- Leukoaraiosis and Disability;
- MET=
- metabolic equivalent;
- MoCA=
- Montreal Cognitive Assessment;
- PURE-Mind=
- Prospective Urban and Rural Epidemiology Mind substudy;
- WMH=
- white matter hyperintensities
Clinical and postmortem studies report variations in risk for dementia despite the same type and degree of brain pathology.1 The factors that lead to this variation in cognition have been termed cognitive reserve.1 Individuals with greater cognitive reserve are hypothesized to have better functioning brains that can tolerate brain pathologic changes without exhibiting impairment.
Most of the research on cognitive reserve has focused on markers that influence the expression of Alzheimer disease pathology in older individuals, with comparatively few studies of cognitive reserve in midlife or of cognitive dysfunction from cerebrovascular pathology.2 A few studies suggest that the association of white matter hyperintensities (WMH) with lower cognition is attenuated in persons with higher education.3 However, whether other potential factors modify the degree of cognitive dysfunction in persons with WMH is not known. Such potential modifiers include social and behavioral function such as level of social engagement, marital status, height, and physical activity.
We analyzed data from 2 large population-based studies: the Canadian Alliance for Healthy Hearts and Healthy Minds (CAHHM) cohort study4 and the Prospective Urban and Rural Epidemiology Mind substudy (PURE-Mind).5 First, we tested the association between MRI vascular brain injury and cognition. Next, we tested the association between cognition and cognitive reserve markers. Finally, we created a cognitive reserve composite score and tested whether the association between vascular brain injury and cognition was modified by high compared to low levels of cognitive reserve, as assessed by the composite score.
Methods
Study Population
This was a cross-sectional analysis of participants enrolled in 2 cohort studies with harmonized assessments of risk factors, MRI, and cognitive outcomes (Figure 1). Our analysis is cross-sectional. Due to the similarities and ability to harmonize data between the 2 cohort studies, we pooled data between the studies. Participants provided written informed consent. The study was approved by local institutional review boards.
(A) MoCA. (B) DSST. Mixed models include random (center) intercepts and fixed effects for age, sex, ethnicity, education, and all variables shown in the figure. CI = confidence interval.
CAHHM is a collaboration of several prospective longitudinal cohort studies that examine socioenvironmental and contextual factors of cardiovascular, cerebrovascular, and subclinical disease and cognition.4 CAHHM includes participants recruited from 2013 to 2019 from 14 sites from 8 previously established study cohorts: the CanPath—Canadian Partnership for Tomorrow's Health (formerly CPTP) Cohorts (BC Generations Project, Alberta's Tomorrow Project, Ontario Health Study, CARTaGENE, and Atlantic PATH6), the Chinese Canadian Alliance cohort, and the Montreal Heart Institute Biobank. Participants were 35–69 years of age.
PURE is a prospective study of the determinants of health in 26 countries.7 For this analysis, additional data were used from Canadian, Chinese, Indian, and Polish participants in the Mind substudy of PURE, who agreed to have a brain MRI and cognitive testing.5 These participants were recruited between 2010 and 2016 from 55 urban and rural Canadian communities from 4 Canadian cities (Vancouver, British Columbia; Hamilton and Ottawa, Ontario; and Quebec City, Quebec); 1 site in Poland (recruiting inhabitants of Lower Silesia); 3 sites in India (in Bangalore, Jaipur, and Thiruvananthapuram); and 1 site in Beijing, China. Participants were age 35–81 and had no prior history of stroke, dementia, or other CNS diseases.
Assessments
Medical history, demographics, lifestyle (individual and household level and neighborhood walkability), anthropometric measures, cognition, and MRI were recorded at enrollment. The CAHHM study was designed to use the same data elements harmonized with PURE to enable combined analyses of the 2 studies.
Markers of cognitive reserve were chosen based on literature reviews and availability of the variables in the database. We specified 6 markers of cognitive reserve: education, involvement in social groups, marital status, level of stress, height, and level of leisure physical activity.1,8,-,11 We dichotomized all variables. Education was classified as college or university, high school or trade school, or less education; social activities were assessed by asking if participants are members of an organized social group; marital status was defined as currently being in a married or common law partnership; stress was dichotomized as moderate or severe stress vs low stress and was captured through a self-reported questionnaire previously used in the INTERHEART study,12 where moderate or severe stress consisted of several periods of work or home stress, or permanent work or home stress; height was stratified by sex and dichotomized at sex-specific medians of above or below 163 cm for females and above or below 176 cm for males (we included height since prior literature suggests that it may have protective effects against dementia13; we hypothesize it may be a proxy for early-life development); leisure physical activity was captured based on self-report of number of hours per week or the equivalent number of metabolic equivalent (MET) minutes (≥960 METs) on the International Physical Activity Questionnaire14 and dichotomized as moderate to high (more than 4 hours per week or the equivalent number of MET minutes [≥960 METs]) vs low. The social activities question was not assessed in the Chinese cohort; therefore, the 466 participants from China (4.2%) were omitted from the analysis of cognitive reserve and the interaction of cognitive reserve with vascular brain injury. While brain imaging and cognition were measured at the same visit in all cohorts, some risk factors related to cognitive reserve were measured at a prior visit. In the PURE study (n = 3,880), assessments were done a median of 6.7 years prior to the MRI and cognitive assessment (interquartile range 3.8–8.6 years), while in the CAHHM cohorts the cognitive reserve variables were captured at the time of the MRI and cognitive assessment, except for education and marital status, which were assessed a median of 5.0 years prior (interquartile range 4.0–6.0 years).
Cognition was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th Edition, Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST)15,16 in both studies. Participants were provided 120 seconds to complete the DSST. DSST was not administered in the Chinese cohort. MoCA and DSST were treated as continuous variables for this analysis. To explore potential associations with cognitive subdomains, MoCA questions were grouped into 6 domains (memory, executive function, visuospatial function, language, attention, and orientation) based on a prior publication.17
The MRI acquisition measures were harmonized between the 2 studies. In the CAHHM study, only high-resolution T1-weighted and T2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) sequences were acquired. In the PURE-Mind study, the following sequences were acquired: high-resolution T1-weighted, dual echo proton density/T2-weighted, T2*-weighted gradient recalled echo, and FLAIR. The MRI sequences in these studies have been validated as part of the Canadian Dementia Imaging Protocol.18 There were 5,075 participants scanned at 1.5T and 5,841 scanned at 3.0T.
MRI data were analyzed by a combined central core laboratory at the University of Calgary and Sunnybrook Hospital, Toronto, Canada. Each scan was visually reviewed for adequate quality. Covert brain infarcts (CBIs) and WMH were defined according to consensus criteria. CBI was defined as brain infarcts that were not associated with overt clinical symptoms of stroke19 and classified as lacunar and nonlacunar. WMH was rated according to the Fazekas scale, in which a score of 0–3 is assigned separately to periventricular and subcortical WMH (sum 0–6), which has been validated against quantitative measurements. High WMH was defined as Fazekas sum score of 4–6. Vascular brain injury was defined as the presence of nonlacunar infarcts or high WMH.
Statistical Analysis
Linear mixed models with random intercepts were used to determine the relationship between brain abnormalities (nonlacunar brain infarcts, WMH, and nonlacunar infarcts) and cognition (MoCA and DSST scores). Site was included as a random intercept variable to account for site-related variations in cognition and differences in the MRI scanner. MoCA and DSST were treated as a continuous variable for analysis. Presence or absence of covert brain infarcts were recorded and WMH was categorized as high (Fazekas total score of 4–6) vs low burden. We evaluated the association of nonlacunar CBI, high WMH, and the composite (vascular brain injury) with cognition, both unadjusted and adjusted for age, sex, and ethnicity as fixed effects. Next, we considered the association of the cognitive reserve variables on the cognitive scores, each separately and all combined in 1 model. Based on the relative size of the β coefficients from the fully adjusted model controlling for all cognitive reserve variables, a cognitive reserve composite score was created where the relative contribution of each marker was based on its effect on cognition as follows: 1 point each for participating in social groups, being in a marital partnership, being taller than the sex-specific median, and having moderate or higher leisure physical activity level; and up to 4 points were assigned for education (2 points for completing high school or trade school and 4 points for a university degree). Education was accorded more points than the other factors because it had a stronger estimated effect on cognition. To test for effect modification, we created models of cognition that included an interaction term between the composite cognitive reserve score and the presence of vascular brain injury. In these models, the cognitive reserve composite variables were centered on the median so that the β coefficient for vascular brain injury represents a participant with the median cognitive reserve. Participants from China (where the social participation question was not asked) and those with missing data on covariates (423/10,450 [4.0%]) were excluded from the model; characteristics of participants with and without missing covariate data are shown in eTable 1 in the Supplement (available from Zenodo, doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4919053). Several sensitivity analyses were undertaken to verify the robustness of the results: (1) including both lacunar and nonlacunar infarcts in the definition of vascular brain injury, (2) removing height from the cognitive reserve variable score, and (3) modeling education alone as the cognitive reserve proxy. To explore the effects of cognitive reserve on MoCA subdomains, logistic regression models were created to estimate the association of vascular brain injury, cognitive reserve composite score, and their interaction on the odds of impairment, defined as losing points on any MoCA questions within that subdomain, and adjusting also for age, sex, and education. A p value of less than 0.05 was considered significant; no adjustment was made for multiple hypothesis testing. Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS v9.4 (SAS Institute).
Data Availability
Anonymized data will be shared by request from a qualified investigator.
Standard Protocol Approvals, Registrations, and Patient Consents
Written informed consent was obtained from the participants. The study was approved by local institutional review boards at each participating site.
Results
Characteristics of the Study Population
Of 11,013 CAHHM and PURE-Mind participants without a clinical history of stroke, we excluded 59 (0.5%) with incomplete MRI sequences and 38 (0.3%) with missing data on cognitive function, leaving 10,916 (99.1%) for analysis (7,036 from CAHHM and 3,880 from PURE). Of the PURE participants, 2,499 were from Canada, 814 from Poland, 466 from China, and 101 from India. Characteristics of the study population are shown in Table 1.
Characteristics of the Study Population
Of the 10,916 total participants included in the analysis, 6,094 (55.8%) were women, mean age was 58.1 ± 8.8 years, and 68.5% were college/university educated. The prevalence of CBI was 5.6% and high WMH 6.3%. Most (72.8%) of the participants had a MoCA score ≥26. The mean MoCA score was 26.7 (SD 2.6) and the mean DSST score was 71.5 (SD 16.8).
Association of Vascular Brain Injury With Cognition
After adjusting for sex, age, ethnicity, education, and study center, the presence of high WMH and nonlacunar CBI were associated with both lower MoCA and DSST scores (Table 2).
Association of Brain Variables With Cognition
Association of Socio-behavioral Variables, Height, and Physical Activity With Cognition
Of the 6 cognitive reserve variables, education had the strongest association with cognition after adjustment for the other 5 cognitive reserve variables (Figure 1). Of the other 5 variables, 3 (participating in social groups, being in a marital partnership, being taller [for both sexes]) were associated with higher MoCA, and 4 (participating in social groups, being in a marital partnership, being taller [for both sexes], more leisure physical activity) were associated with higher DSST (Figure 1).
Modifying Effects of Cognitive Reserve Variables on the Association of Vascular Brain Injury With Cognition
Table 3 and Figure 2 show the modifying effects of the cognitive reserve composite score on the relationships between vascular brain injury and cognition. Vascular brain injury (either nonlacunar CBI or high WMH) was independently associated with lower scores on the MoCA (p < 0 .001) and DSST (p < 0.0001). Higher cognitive reserve composite score was associated with higher scores on both DSST (p < 0.0001) and MoCA (p < 0.0001). However, the association of vascular brain injury with MoCA was not modified by the cognitive reserve composite score (β coefficient 0.03, 95% confidence interval [CI] −0.07 to 0.13, interaction p = 0.59). Similarly, the association between vascular brain injury and DSST was not modified by the cognitive reserve composite score (β coefficient 0.10, 95% CI −0.46 to 0.67, interaction p = 0.72).
Modifying Effect of Cognitive Reserve Composite Score on the Association Between Vascular Brain Injury (VBI) and Cognitive Scores
Association between composite cognitive reserve score and cognition ([A] Montreal Cognitive Assessment [MoCA] and [B] Digit Symbol Substitution Test [DSST]) in the presence (dashed line) and absence (solid line) of vascular brain injury, with p values for the interaction between cognitive reserve score and vascular brain injury. Gray bands indicate 95% confidence limits. Higher cognitive reserve score was associated with higher cognitive function and vascular brain injury was associated with lower cognitive function (see Table 3 for details). However, there was no interaction between cognitive reserve score and vascular brain injury for either MoCA or DSST as evidenced by the nearly parallel slopes and nonsignificant tests of interaction.
Sensitivity Analyses
The associations between vascular brain injury, cognitive reserve proxies, and cognition were essentially unchanged in sensitivity analyses (full results available at Zenodo, doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4919053) including lacunar infarcts (Supplemental eTable 2), excluding height (Supplemental eTable 3), or using education alone as the proxy for cognitive reserve (Supplemental eTable 4). In analyses of MoCA subdomains (Supplemental eTable 5), the cognitive reserve composite score was associated with all subdomains, while the vascular brain injury variable was associated with impairments in memory, executive function, visuospatial function, and language, but not attention or orientation. There was no evidence for effect modification by cognitive reserve in any of these models (Supplemental eTables 2–5).
Discussion
We examined the modifying effects of markers of cognitive reserve on the associations between cerebrovascular brain injury and cognition. As expected, nonlacunar infarcts and higher WMH were associated with lower cognition. Five of the 6 markers of cognitive reserve—high education (most strongly), greater height, greater leisure physical activity, being in a marital partnership, and participation in social groups—were independently associated with better cognition (leisure physical activity was only significant for DSST). It is possible that leisure physical activity was only associated with DSST and not MoCA because DSST is a more sensitive and continuous measure, resulting in greater sensitivity to detect smaller effects on cognition. The composite cognitive reserve score, based on these 5 significant reserve variables, was also associated with better cognition. However, contrary to our prespecified hypothesis, we failed to find evidence that these cognitive reserve markers modified the effect of covert vascular brain injury (defined as either nonlacunar infarcts or high WMH) on cognition.
Of the few studies on cognitive reserve in persons with cerebral small vessel disease, research has mostly focused on education as a modifier of the relationship between WMH and cognition.20 A prior review identified 4 studies in the general population that found that the adverse effect of WMH on cognition was attenuated in persons with higher education.21,-,24 A more recent publication from the Leukoaraiosis and Disability (LADIS) study showed that the 3-year rate of cognitive decline in persons with high WMH was slower in persons with high education compared to low education.3 Two studies found that the association of CBI on cognition was attenuated in persons with higher education.25,26 There are few studies on markers other than education. In the LADIS study, the association of WMH with 3-year rate of cognitive decline was attenuated in persons with higher occupational attainment, defined as a white collar or professional occupation.3 Another study found that the association of high WMH with lower cognition was attenuated by high cognitive leisure activity such as playing board games, social activities, or doing puzzles.27 This shows the need for datasets that evaluate the effects of cognitive reserve variables other than education alone.
These previous studies suggested that higher cognitive reserve may attenuate the negative effects of cerebral small vessel disease on cognition, but we did not observe this in our study. The reason for the differences in study findings are not clear but could relate to different cohort characteristics. Compared to other studies, our cohort was younger (mean age 58 compared to >65 for the other studies), more contemporary (recruited in 2010–2019 instead of the 1990s to early 2000s), and more highly educated, with the majority having a college or university education. We also examined more cognitive reserve variables compared to other studies, providing a more holistic approach to cognitive reserve that intends to capture a variety of cognitive reserve markers.
Although we did not find strong evidence for effect modification, we did find that lifestyle and social factors were independently associated with higher cognition regardless of the presence of MRI-defined vascular brain injury. Education is well known to be associated with higher cognition and low education is a risk factor for dementia. Higher physical activity has been associated with lower risk for dementia28 although the extent to which exercise programs prevent dementia is unclear.29 Higher social participation and avoidance of social isolation and loneliness may protect against dementia, with 41% increased relative risk in persons with low social participation.30 Being in a marital partnership has been associated with better cognition and lower risk of dementia; in a recent systematic review, being single throughout life was associated with 40% increased risk.31 High psychological stress, including stress in midlife, has been associated with 60% increased risk for later-life dementia.32 Greater height does not directly influence cognition but could be a proxy for healthier early-life development, and has been associated with lower risk for dementia in some studies.13 Higher synaptic density and better functioning brain networks are hypothesized to underlie the potential beneficial effects of these factors on cognitive performance and risk for dementia.20
Study strengths include a large population-based sample. As for limitations, although our cross-sectional design allows us to make inferences regarding the modifying effects of cognitive reserve factors on the association between covert cerebrovascular disease and cognition, it does not allow us to determine the temporal association between cognitive reserve factors and change in cognition over the lifespan. Although it is plausible that the cognitive reserve variables cause better cognition, we cannot exclude that the direction of causation is reversed. That is, early cognitive decline might lead to less physical activity, less engagement in social groups, and greater likelihood of being single. This concern for reverse causation is somewhat mitigated as we looked at early subclinical cognitive changes in persons without dementia, where even low cognitive performers were within normal ranges. Longitudinal follow-up of this cohort is planned, in which we can evaluate the long-term risk of dementia according to level of education, social activities, marital status, height, and physical activity. There may be genetic contributors to cognitive reserve, but they have not been analyzed in our study. Future studies should investigate the role that physiologic factors, including cerebrovascular reactivity, play in the development of cognitive reserve.33 Another potential limitation is the use of brief cognitive assessment tools, which consisted of a frequently used cognitive screening tool (MoCA) and a test of processing speed (DSST). However, these tools are well validated, have been used in other population-based studies, and are sensitive to aging, to change over time, and to the presence of covert vascular brain injury. To provide a straightforward clinical interpretation, we dichotomized cut points for our cognitive reserve variables, which could have led to some loss of statistical power.
Our data suggest that enhancing cognitive reserve through better early life education, physical activity, and social engagement may potentially help improve cognition but is not likely to mitigate the deleterious effects of covert cerebrovascular disease. Therefore, an effective dementia prevention strategy will need to promote cognitive-enhancing social and behavioral activities while reducing the risk of incident CBI and WMH progression. This should include individual and health policy interventions to improve early life education and promote active social participation, strong family relationships, and increased physical activity, as well as new strategies to reduce progression of covert cerebrovascular disease.34
Study Funding
Dr. Smith had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. The funders played no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. CAHHM was funded by the Canadian Partnership Against Cancer (CPAC), Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada, and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR). Financial contributions were also received from the Population Health Research Institute. Dr. Sonia Anand received CIHR Foundation grant FDN-143255; Dr. Jack Tu, CIHR Foundation grant FDN-143313; and Dr. Eric Smith, CIHR Foundation grant FDN-154317. In-kind contributions were received from Dr. Alan Moody and Dr. Sandra E. Black from Sunnybrook Hospital, Toronto, for MRI reading costs, and Bayer AG for provision of IV contrast. The Canadian Partnership for Tomorrow Project is funded by CPAC, Genome Quebec, Genome Canada, Ontario Institute for Cancer Research Government of Alberta, and Alberta Cancer Foundation. The Montreal Heart Institute Biobank is funded by André Desmarais and France Chrétien-Desmarais and the Montreal Heart Institute Foundation. The PURE study is an investigator-initiated study that was funded by the Population Health Research Institute, Hamilton Health Sciences Research Institute (HHSRI), the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Heart and Stroke Foundation of Ontario, Support from Canadian Institutes of Health Research's Strategy for Patient Oriented Research, through the Ontario SPOR Support Unit, as well as the Ontario Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care and through unrestricted grants from several pharmaceutical companies (with major contributions from AstraZeneca [Canada], Sanofi-Aventis [France and Canada], Boehringer Ingelheim [Germany and Canada], Servier, and GlaxoSmithKline), and additional contributions from Novartis and King Pharma and from various national or local organizations in participating countries. In Canada, there was additional support for PURE from an unrestricted grant from Dairy Farmers of Canada and the National Dairy Council (US), Public Health Agency of Canada, and Champlain Cardiovascular Disease Prevention Network. The PURE Poland substudy was partially funded by Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education grant 290/W-PURE/2008/0 and the MRI substudy was supported by a Polish National Science Centre grant (NCN 2015/17/B/NZ7-02963).
Disclosure
The authors report no conflicts of interest and no relevant disclosures. Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures.
Appendix Authors

Footnotes
Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article.
The Article Processing Charge was funded by the authors.
- Received February 2, 2021.
- Accepted in final form September 3, 2021.
- Copyright © 2021 The Author(s). Published by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. on behalf of the American Academy of Neurology.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND), which permits downloading and sharing the work provided it is properly cited. The work cannot be changed in any way or used commercially without permission from the journal.
References
- 1.↵
- 2.↵
- Durrani R,
- Hill MD,
- Smith EE
- 3.↵
- Jokinen H,
- Melkas S,
- Madureira S, et al.
- 4.↵
- Anand SS,
- Tu JV,
- Awadalla P, et al.
- 5.↵
- 6.↵
- Dummer TJB,
- Awadalla P,
- Boileau C, et al.
- 7.↵
- 8.↵
- Scarmeas N,
- Levy G,
- Tang MX,
- Manly J,
- Stern Y
- 9.
- 10.↵
- 11.↵
- Hakansson K,
- Rovio S,
- Helkala EL, et al.
- 12.↵
- 13.↵
- Russ TC,
- Kivimaki M,
- Starr JM,
- Stamatakis E,
- Batty GD
- 14.↵
- 15.↵
- 16.↵
- Merz ZC,
- Lace JW,
- Eisenstein AM,
- Grant AF
- 17.↵
- 18.↵
- 19.↵
- 20.↵
- 21.↵
- Dufouil C,
- Alperovitch A,
- Tzourio C
- 22.
- Nebes RD,
- Meltzer CC,
- Whyte EM, et al.
- 23.↵
- 24.↵
- 25.↵
- Elkins JS,
- Longstreth WT Jr..,
- Manolio TA,
- Newman AB,
- Bhadelia RA,
- Johnston SC
- 26.↵
- Farfel JM,
- Nitrini R,
- Suemoto CK, et al.
- 27.↵
- 28.↵
- 29.↵
- 30.↵
- 31.↵
- Sommerlad A,
- Ruegger J,
- Singh-Manoux A,
- Lewis G,
- Livingston G
- 32.↵
- 33.↵
- Siniscalchi A,
- Gray C,
- Malferrari G
- 34.↵
Letters: Rapid online correspondence
REQUIREMENTS
You must ensure that your Disclosures have been updated within the previous six months. Please go to our Submission Site to add or update your Disclosure information.
Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.
If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.
Submission specifications:
- Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
- Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
- Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
- Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
- Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.
You May Also be Interested in
Hastening the Diagnosis of Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis
Dr. Brian Callaghan and Dr. Kellen Quigg
► Watch
Related Articles
- No related articles found.