Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion (DEI)
    • Neurology: Clinical Practice Accelerator
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit New Manuscript
    • Submit Revised Manuscript
    • Author Center

Advanced Search

Main menu

  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion (DEI)
    • Neurology: Clinical Practice Accelerator
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit New Manuscript
    • Submit Revised Manuscript
    • Author Center
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Neurology
Home
The most widely read and highly cited peer-reviewed neurology journal
  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
Site Logo
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

Share

August 17, 2021; 97 (7 Supplement 1) Patient Reported Outcomes

Recommendations for Measurement of Attention Outcomes in Preschoolers With Neurofibromatosis

View ORCID ProfileBonita P. Klein-Tasman, Kristin Lee, Heather L. Thompson, Jennifer Janusz, View ORCID ProfileJonathan M. Payne, Sara Pardej, View ORCID ProfilePeter de Blank, Tess Kennedy, Kelly M. Janke, Allison del Castillo, View ORCID ProfileKarin S. Walsh, on behalf of the REiNS International Collaboration
First published July 6, 2021, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000012423
Bonita P. Klein-Tasman
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Bonita P. Klein-Tasman
Kristin Lee
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Heather L. Thompson
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jennifer Janusz
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jonathan M. Payne
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Jonathan M. Payne
Sara Pardej
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Peter de Blank
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Peter de Blank
Tess Kennedy
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kelly M. Janke
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Allison del Castillo
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Karin S. Walsh
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Karin S. Walsh
From the Department of Psychology (B.P.K.-T., K.L., S.P.), University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee; Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders (H.L.T.), California State University, Sacramento; University of Colorado School of Medicine (J.J.), Aurora; Murdoch Children's Research Institute and Department of Pediatrics (J.M.P.), University of Melbourne, Australia; University of Cincinnati Medical Center (P.d.B.), OH; Children's National Hospital (T.K., A.d.C., K.S.W.), Gilbert NF Institute, Washington, DC; and Division of Oncology (K.M.J.), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA.
Full PDF
Citation
Recommendations for Measurement of Attention Outcomes in Preschoolers With Neurofibromatosis
Bonita P. Klein-Tasman, Kristin Lee, Heather L. Thompson, Jennifer Janusz, Jonathan M. Payne, Sara Pardej, Peter de Blank, Tess Kennedy, Kelly M. Janke, Allison del Castillo, Karin S. Walsh, on behalf of the REiNS International Collaboration
Neurology Aug 2021, 97 (7 Supplement 1) S81-S90; DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012423

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Permissions

Make Comment

See Comments

Downloads
583

Share

  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Loading

Abstract

Children with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) are at increased risk for attention problems. While most research has been conducted with school-aged cohorts, preschool-aged children offer a novel developmental window for clinical studies, with the promise that treatments implemented earlier in the developmental trajectory may most effectively modify risk for later difficulties. Designing research studies around the youngest children with NF1 can result in intervention earlier in the developmental cascade associated with NF1 gene abnormalities. Furthermore, clinical trials for medications targeting physical and psychological aspects of NF1 often include individuals spanning a wide age range, including preschool-aged children. In a prior report, the REiNS Neurocognitive Subcommittee made recommendations regarding performance-based and observer-rated measures of attention for use in clinical trials and highlighted the need for separate consideration of assessment methods for young children. The observer-rated Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale–Preschool version is recommended as a primary outcome measure. The NIH Toolbox Flanker, Dimensional Change Card Sort, and List Sort Working Memory tasks and Digits Forward from the Differential Ability Scales–2nd Edition (performance-based measures) are recommended as secondary outcome measures. Specific methodologic recommendations for inclusion of preschoolers in clinical trials research are also offered.

Children with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) are at increased risk for attention problems.1,2 While most research has been conducted with school-aged cohorts, preschool-aged children offer a novel developmental window for clinical studies, with the promise that treatments implemented earlier in the developmental trajectory may most effectively modify risk for later difficulties. Designing research studies around the youngest children with NF1 can result in intervention earlier in the developmental cascade associated with NF1 gene abnormalities. Furthermore, clinical trials for medications targeting physical and psychological aspects of NF1 often include individuals spanning a wide age range, including preschool-aged children. In a prior report, the Response Evaluation in Neurofibromatosis and Schwannomatosis (REiNS) Neurocognitive Subcommittee made recommendations regarding performance-based and observer-rated measures of attention for use in clinical trials3 and highlighted the need for separate consideration of assessment methods for young children. In this article, we report the activities of the REiNS Preschool Group of the REiNS Neurocognitive Subcommittee, including review and recommendations of observer-rated and performance-based measures of attention and related emerging executive functioning for use with young children in clinical trials.

The preschool years are a time of rapid development of a range of cognitive skills, and there is considerable variability among the general population of preschoolers in their attention and related emerging executive functioning skills. For the purposes of this report, attention can be defined as the conscious focus of awareness on certain aspects of the environment/other/self while excluding other stimuli.4 While separate components of attention and executive functioning are described for older children, adolescents, and adults,5 these constructs are less well differentiated among preschool-aged children.6 Across development, however, attention is central to human functioning and is foundational to a range of abilities that are areas of vulnerability for children with NF1, including acquisition of preacademic and academic skills7 and perception of social cues to support social functioning.8

Research About Attention and Emerging Executive Functioning in Children With NF1

A small body of work examines attention and emerging executive functioning in children with NF1 (table 1 provides a listing of published studies, sample sizes, and related findings). It is notable that the focus on preschool-aged children is new within the NF1 field. While studies before 2010 may have included a few children <6 years of age in their samples, it is only in the last decade that studies focused specifically on younger children have been published. Across these studies, some patterns emerge with respect to measurement approaches and findings. First, the most commonly used observer-reported measures are the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning–Preschool (BRIEF-P),9 Behavior Assessment System for Children–2nd Edition (BASC-2),10 and the Conners11 Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Parent Report Short Form (CPRS-R; also referred to as the CADS in some literature). One study used the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist,12 without a specific focus on attention.13

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1

Literature Including Attention and Emerging Executive Functioning in Young Children With NF1

The findings from the studies focused on preschoolers with NF1 generally converge on a pattern of relatively subtle attention difficulties in the preschool years at the group level. There is variability across studies in the methods used to assess attention difficulties and in the degree of attention difficulties identified among young children with NF1. Very few studies include respondents other than parents, possibly related to variability in school attendance among preschoolers. Furthermore, there is no current published literature on the predictive utility of attention measures for later functioning for children with NF1 or on the stability of identified attention problems from the preschool to later years.

Findings with the BASC-2, which is a broad measure that includes subscales assessing attention and hyperactivity, are variable. Sangster et al.14 and Lorenzo et al.15,16 did not find evidence for difficulties with attention. Brei et al.17 found greater attention problems in the NF1 group compared to normative data, while Klein-Tasman et al.18 (with an overlapping sample seen at a different longitudinal data point) found a medium effect size that did not reach statistical significance compared to an unaffected participant group.

Findings with the CPRS-R, the parent-report measure used most to assess attention difficulties within the NF1 literature, also are not consistent across studies. Sangster et al.14 did not identify elevations in either inattention or broader ADHD symptoms using this measure. In contrast, with overlapping samples of participants, Brei et al.17 and Casnar et al.19 identified elevated scores relative to the normative mean on many indices of this measure, with some evidence suggesting that inattentive symptomatology may be more discrepant from the normative mean than the hyperactivity symptomatology in the preschool years. On the BRIEF-P, a measure of emerging everyday executive functioning that includes attention-related behaviors, overall composite scores are generally not indicative of difficulties among preschoolers with NF1.14,16,20 However, the BRIEF-P Working Memory scale emerges as one that appears to be particularly effective at identifying difficulties compared to population means or unaffected contrast groups14,20 according to both parent and teacher report.20 This is the scale with the most overlap with attention problems in the behaviors assessed.

The use of performance-based measures has much less consistency in the literature, with individual laboratories using different measures (table 1). Studies using performance-based measures are sometimes indicative of challenges at the group level (e.g., on the Shape School,21 Kiddie Continuous Performance Test [K-CPT],22 and Differential Ability Scales–2nd Edition [DAS-II] Recall of Digits Forward)23 but sometimes not (e.g., NEPSY Visual Attention,24 Tower of Hanoi,25 Delayed Alternation26).

Little is known about the natural history of attention from preschool to school-age years in children with NF1. There are few cross-sectional developmental studies including children with NF1 in both the preschool and school-age years and none that report specific preschool-to school-age effects on attention. There is also very little longitudinal work tracking attention in the same children with NF1 over time from preschool to school age. One notable exception is the work by Rietman et al.,27 who found no significant change in externalizing symptomatology on the Child Behavior Checklist over time in their longitudinal investigation.

Clinical Trials Related to Attention in Preschoolers

The REiNS Neurocognitive Preschool Group examined the current clinical trials literature related to attention in preschoolers more broadly to assist in identifying measures that may be useful for this purpose in children with NF1. We began by conducting a search of trials on ClinicalTrials.gov (including search terms of ADHD, attention, preschool, early childhood) followed by a parallel systematic literature search using PubMed to identify related published data. We made note of measures that had been used in at least 2 clinical trials. Several interventional trials focused specifically on improving attention in the preschool years. The vast majority used observer-rated measures, including the BRIEF-P28 (NCT01675869; NCT02225236), Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist29 (NCT01795040), ADHD–Rating Scale (ADHD-RS-P30 or ADHD-RS31; NCT00254462; NCT02642666; NCT01684644; NCT01918436; NCT02677519), Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham Rating Scale version IV32 (NCT02433145; NCT00856063), Children's Global Assessment Scale33 (NCT00031395; NCT00517647), Conners Parent/Teacher Rating Scale33,34 (NCT00517647; NCT00018863; NCT01320098), BASC-235 (NCT01919073), Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory35,36 (NCT01919073; NCT03967509), Conners Early Childhood (EC)37,38 (NCT02677519), and Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory39 (NCT01547702; NCT03806946). Most of these studies reported improvements in ADHD-related symptoms in response to intervention, which includes both psychopharmacologic and psychosocial approaches.

More recently, clinical trials have begun including computerized measures of attention and executive ability, including the KiTAP40,41 (NCT02642666), CPT42 (NCT02807870), and Minnesota Executive Function Scale43 (NCT03383172). There are currently very few clinical trials that have published data using computerized measures. It should be noted that many registered studies on ClinicalTrials.gov did not yet have published data available. It is also notable that there were very few trials for attention that spanned a wider age range but included preschoolers. Most studies that included preschoolers focused specifically on the preschool years. In sum, most clinical trials that include preschoolers use observer-rated measures of attention problems based on DSM criteria for ADHD. The most commonly used measure is a version of the ADHD-RS. The number of studies currently using computerized measures is small, but a review of registered trials suggests that it is possible that their use is on the rise.

The aims of this work were to detail the strengths and weaknesses of candidate attention outcome measures based on currently published data to arrive at recommendations for NF1 clinical trial endpoints and to discuss special considerations when assessing attention in young children with NF1, with the goal of providing practical guidelines for clinical trials procedures.

Methods

The REiNS Neurocognitive Preschool Group reviewed a targeted set of performance-based and observer-rated measures of attention and emerging executive functions for consideration for use in clinical trials that include preschoolers with NF1. We reviewed measures that were deemed promising from the review of the intervention literature with preschoolers without NF1, measures that are commonly used to measure attention in preschool-aged children with NF1, and additional measures of attention as nominated by members of the group on the basis of their experience and expertise. Some measures were excluded from further consideration because they did not meet basic threshold for review (table 2). Group consensus was that these tools were used in very few studies, offered no continuity with measures for older children, were accompanied by outdated psychometric data, were experimental measures without normative data, or were not feasible to administer within a clinical trials context (except with specialized personnel). Notably, researchers might find some of these measures useful as ancillary outcomes should they have personnel with sufficient expertise or if the focus of the clinical trial is restricted to preschool-aged children.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2

Attention-Related Measures That Did Not Receive Full Review

Measures that met the basic threshold underwent a full review and were rated by the full group. These are listed in tables 3 and 4. To systematically evaluate these measures, we used the same rating form used in prior work from the REiNS Neurocognitive Subcommittee.3 Each measure was rated on 6 criteria: (1) patient characteristics (age range, use with specific populations); (2) use in published studies (descriptive and clinical trials); (3) domains assessed; (4) availability of standard scores; (5) psychometric properties; and (6) feasibility for use in clinical trials. The same procedures as those outlined by Walsh et al.3 in table 3 were used here, with a conclusion about each measure based largely on the Patient Characteristics, Psychometrics, and Feasibility category ratings.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3

Age Range, Evaluation, and Ratings of Observer-Rated Measures That Received Full Review

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 4

Age Range, Evaluation, and Ratings of Performance-Based Measures That Received Full Review

Results

Table 3 lists ratings of the observer-rated measures evaluated, including central strengths and weaknesses of each observer-rated measure. Table 4 lists ratings of the performance-based measures evaluated by the group, including central strengths and weaknesses of each measure.

Discussion

On the basis of the systematic review of performance-based and observer-rated measures of attention for use in the preschool years, the REiNS Neurocognitive Preschool Group converged on several recommendations for specific measure choices and consensus regarding the status of the current literature and suggested methodologic and practical processes (the figure gives a summary of the main points).

Figure
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure Summary of Main Points

ADHD-RS-EC = Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale for Early Childhood; DAS-II = Differential Ability Scales–2nd Edition; DCCS = Dimensional Change Card Sort; LSWM = List Sort Working Memory; NF1 = neurofibromatosis type 1.

Consensus About Measures

In the assessment of observer-rated measures, the group discussed a range of theoretical and practical considerations. For example, there was discussion of the degree to which separate indices of attention and hyperactivity are needed on questionnaire measures. There was consensus that, especially because clinical trials might be targeting different aspects of ADHD-related symptomatology, ideally observer-rated measures would provide separate indications of hyperactivity and inattention. A related consideration was whether the measure needed to map onto diagnostic criteria for ADHD. There was consensus that this might not be critical given that we are expecting dimensional rather than categorical changes in symptomatology with intervention; in other words, it is not necessarily expected that attention problems would normalize (i.e., no longer meeting diagnostic criteria for ADHD) but rather that treatment will result in improvement in ADHD-related symptomatology.

The CPRS-R and the ADHD-RS-P were rated highest for the assessment of ADHD-related symptomatology. The CPRS-R has been used in research with preschool-aged children with NF1.14,17,19 The main limitation was that this measure is no longer supported by the publisher and therefore updated norms are not being collected. The Conners EC, while sharing a similar name, is a substantially different measure. It combines both attention and hyperactivity items into 1 scale, and rather than having 1 outcome appropriate for children ages 4 through 18 years (as in the case of the CPRS-R), there is a need to change from the Conners EC to the Conners-3 at 6 years of age. Group consensus was to recommend the ADHD-RS-P. The measure was recommended for several reasons: (1) it is commonly used in clinical trials research related to attention problems; (2) it is very similar to the CPRS-R (which is the measure most widely used in NF1) in its close alignment with diagnostic criteria for ADHD; and (3) the preschool version uses largely the same items as the version for older children (ADHD-RS) with only slight alterations in the language used to make it developmentally appropriate,30 allowing consistent assessment into the school-age years. The ADHD-RS-P is therefore recommended as a primary outcome measure for clinical trials including preschoolers.

In the assessment of performance-based measures, the NIH Toolbox tasks (Flanker, Dimensional Change Card Sort, List Sort Working Memory) and the K-CPT-2 emerged as the most promising tools for use as attention endpoints in NF1 clinical trials research. The NIH Toolbox is heavily grounded in empirical research about the nature of attention and emerging executive function, has strong normative groups, has been used in large samples of clinical groups, and is increasingly used in research, including clinical trials.44 Limitations discussed regarding the NIH Toolbox measures included that while the Flanker and Dimensional Change Card Sort tasks are considered central to attention and emerging executive functioning by developmental and experimental literature,45 the NIH Toolbox does not include a traditional Continuous Performance Test, a more commonly used clinical measure of attention and impulsivity, and there is no current published literature using these measures with children with NF1. Strengths of the K-CPT-2 include that its predecessor, the K-CPT, has been used widely to characterize attention in preschoolers and has also captured attention problems in preschoolers with NF1. Limitations discussed regarding the K-CPT-2 included that the published literature uses a prior version of this measure (K-CPT) and that there are limited published data on the concurrence between scores on the K-CPT and the corresponding measure to be used in older children (CPT-342). Changes from the prior version include an expansion of the appropriate age range (from 4 to 5 for the K-CPT to 4 to 7 for the K-CPT-2) and an expansion of the kinds of scores available to include additional dependent variables. For the K-CPT, the group also recommended that the Commissions and Omissions scores from the K-CPT-2 are used as the main endpoints, which appear to be the most valid and reliable outcomes of this measure. It is notable and important, however, that there is often a tradeoff between these 2 scores: as Commissions score increases, Omissions score decreases and vice versa.46 An additional approach would be to use reaction time variability,47 although there is little literature on the validity of this dependent measure for use with young children. Overall, the group consensus was to recommend the NIH Toolbox measures as secondary outcomes for clinical trials including preschoolers because there is little published literature using the KCPT-2 or about the correspondence between the KCPT and the KCPT-2 and because the NIH Toolbox has the advantages of continuity across a wide age range and increasing use with individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders and in clinical trials. Further research into the psychometrics properties of both of these measures with young children with NF1 is needed.

In the work of the REiNS Neurocognitive Subcommittee, a digit span measure was recommended as a secondary endpoint. For conceptual continuity, DAS–II Recall of Digits Forward, a forward digit span task appropriate for use in the preschool years,23 was also reviewed and is recommended. Unlike many other digit span measures, digits are presented 2 per second, more closely approximating the speed of speech. This measure has shown relations to parent ratings of Working Memory from the BRIEF-P in young children with NF1.20 The main drawbacks of this measure include cost (in that that it is available only as part of the whole DAS-II) and that training is required to ensure accurate administration.

These recommendations differ somewhat from those for measurement of attention in older children with NF1.3 Here, the ADHD-RS-P is recommended (rather than a Conners scale) because the Conners EC includes only a combined attention and hyperactivity score. In contrast, the ADHD-RS (for school-age children) and ADHD-RS-P (for preschool-aged children) are largely parallel. Here, the DAS-II Digits Forward was recommended rather than a Wechsler Scale digit span because the preschool Wechsler does not include a digit span, and the DAS-II Digits Forward task can be used into the school-age years with norms until 17 years 11 months. Hence, our recommendation is that if a clinical trial plans to include preschool-aged children, investigators should use these measures so that they can remain consistent into the school-age years. The recommendations remain as earlier proposed for trials that do not include preschool-aged children.3

Methodologic and Practical Considerations for Clinical Trials Including Preschoolers With NF1

It is not clear how best to measure attention longitudinally when participants span preschool and school ages during the course of a clinical trial. There was discussion about how centrally to consider continuity of preschool measures with those used in the school-aged years in our recommendations. For clinical trials research, children are seen more than once, and in the case of NF1, they may be seen over a lengthy period (e.g., 2 years in the case of the MEK inhibitor study [NCT02096471]). This is different from traditional ADHD medication trials, which typically span a much shorter treatment period.48 For many observer-rated measures, there are different forms normed for the preschool years than for the school-age years (e.g., BASC-2 and BASC-3, BRIEF-P/BRIEF-2). Similarly, some of the performance-based tasks reviewed change versions with age. This makes sense because there are different relevant behaviors and functions at different ages. However, it presents challenges for longitudinal work because, when children are followed up over time, they may move from 1 normative group to another. There was consensus that this is a critical consideration and that there are very few relevant data that address anticipated stability or change in scores when a child moves from 1 age range form to another.

There are several approaches when conducting longitudinal work. One approach is to stay with the initial measure so that it is possible to examine change in item or scale endorsement over time with the same item set. For example, if the child entered the study at age 5 years and was given the preschool form or test and then turns 6 by the next scheduled assessment, they could be given the preschool form or test again even if that is not the one that is suggested for their age because raw score change could be examined. When a study has a shorter timeline, this can be a feasible approach because arguably even the normative data may be appropriate to use from a slightly younger chronologic age. Another approach is to administer the age-appropriate form and to examine change based on analogous scales. When there is a longer time period between assessments, this may be especially appropriate because, for example, administering a preschool measure to an 8-year-old is likely not appropriate from a content validity standpoint. This creates some interpretive challenges, however, because there may not be sufficient data to conclude that the analogous scales are fully comparable. If there is a control group, however, this is less of a problem because it will be possible to examine whether the amount of change observed for the intervention group is different from the amount of change seen across forms for a group who did not receive intervention. Hence, when clinical trial participants are expected to span 2 age-appropriate measures, a control group will be essential to gauge the meaning of change in the scores on these measures. Furthermore, it is imperative that the questionnaire respondent remains consistent across time for each participant and that assistance be provided for questionnaire completion for any respondents who do not have at least a middle school education. Consultation with a psychologist regarding outcome choices is recommended to carefully consider the options and to assist in choosing the most appropriate methodology for a given trial.

The members of this group also encourage test developers and validators to consider addressing this question by conorming measures in an overlapping age range to establish relations between scores on the preschool measure and scores on the school-age measure. For example, it would be greatly beneficial to know whether the T score of a late 7-year-old administered the KCPT-2 (appropriate for ages 4–7 years) would be similar to their T score if they were administered the CPT-3 soon after they turned 8 years old.

Some special considerations warrant attention when preschoolers are included in clinical trials. First, it is important to keep in mind the inherent variability in attention in the preschool years. One of the reasons it may be challenging to capture attention problems in young children with NF1 is that the range of attention functioning in the normative population is wide. Therefore, attention problems may emerge in children with NF1 only in the school-aged years, not because the difficulties were not present when they were younger but rather because, when they were younger, these difficulties appeared to be within the broad range of attention that is typically seen in the general population. Furthermore, preschoolers have variable experience with structured classroom settings in which demands for attention are more likely to be present. Some children have not yet had the chance to practice paying attention when they participate in research while others may have. Relatedly, some parents may not have noticed attention problems because there has not been a need for the children to engage in sustained attention, and other caregiver observation may not be available for all children. Furthermore, within the normative developmental literature, there is evidence that the domains of attention may be less differentiated among young children.6 Working memory and inhibition difficulties may be the first to be evident, while challenges with sustained attention and organization may be harder to capture in the preschool years.49 This does not necessarily point to the need for different measures across development (because the measures used in the school-age period also assess inhibition), but it may mean that the manifestations of attention problems may vary with age, such that different dependent measures might capture the difficulties at different ages. Studies of the predictive utility of a range of attention measures from the preschool years to the school-age years are needed in case there are subtle or specific attention problem indicators that may be especially predictive of later attention problems in children with NF1, who are at particular risk for attention problems. Last, additional work outlining the domains of cognitive functioning that are most critical to capture for a sense of functioning in preschoolers with NF1 is needed to ensure that the most relevant outcomes for preschoolers are included in clinical trials. Lifespan studies of attention in NF1 are especially needed.

Second, there are practical considerations in the assessment of attention in preschoolers.50,51 If the outcome measure is not an observer-rated questionnaire measure, researchers will need to be aware that assessment of preschool-aged children requires expertise that may not be as necessary for the assessment of older children. Preschool-aged children may need more explanation and support to fully grasp the task requirements. They may need specific reinforcers and flexibility in testing (e.g., stickers along a path to a larger prize to motivate them; allowing for some movement). An upbeat, engaging interpersonal style may be important to maintain engagement, yet it is critical that the examiner not provide so much structure that they adversely affect the validity of the measure. Specific procedures to address off-task behavior or task challenges should be delineated (e.g., how many times to run through practice trials before deciding that the child cannot complete the task, how many times to provide reminders to attend to the task, what to do if the child gets up from their seat and walks away). Warm but directive statements may be important (e.g., rather than “do you want to sit here?” warmly saying “OK, sit right here!”). Frequent breaks and alternation between more challenging and less challenging tasks may be especially needed. Most critically, clear standardization of these procedures between testers and across multiple sites is important for clinical trials research. To this end, these measures must be administered by professionals experienced with the use of these performance measures among preschoolers.

In summary, was consensus that additional research about attention in the preschool years, including related concepts such as emerging executive functioning, is needed to inform the approach to measurement of attention in children with NF1. Furthermore, additional clarity about the expected developmental trajectory of attention in children with NF1 would be especially helpful. Any change in attention in an intervention study needs to be interpreted against the backdrop of expected developmental trajectory patterns for children with NF1. Questionnaire measures appear to be the most used approach to capturing changes in attention in clinical trials and are likely to be useful for children with NF1 as well. While computerized measures have been used sparsely in the NF1 literature to date, they may be promising for clinical trials research with NF1 because they are often designed with multiple administrations in mind and are relatively easy to administer. It would likely be useful to include both approaches within a clinical trial to effectively capture a child's functioning and intervention-related change. Studies examining the correspondence between these different measurement modalities generally show limited convergence for children with and children without NF1.50 There is also emerging evidence that the use of multiple measures may result in better reliability of outcomes that may set the stage for greater sensitivity to change.52 Additional research into the psychometric properties of measures of attention and the relative utility of performance and observer-rated measures in preschoolers with NF1 is needed, especially as new measures are developed and published.

Study Funding

The authors report no targeted funding.

Disclosure

The authors have no disclosures. Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures.

Acknowledgment

The authors acknowledge the support of the Children's Tumor Foundation for the REiNS International Collaboration.

Appendix Authors

Table

Footnotes

  • Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article.

  • Received October 9, 2020.
  • Accepted in final form April 30, 2021.
  • © 2021 American Academy of Neurology

References

  1. 1.↵
    1. Hyman SL,
    2. Shores A,
    3. North KN
    . The nature and frequency of cognitive deficits in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Neurology. 2005;65(7):1037-1044.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. 2.↵
    1. Lehtonen A,
    2. Howie E,
    3. Trump D,
    4. Huson SM
    . Behaviour in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: cognition, executive function, attention, emotion, and social competence: review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(2):111-125.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. 3.↵
    1. Walsh KS,
    2. Janusz J,
    3. Wolters PL, et al
    . Neurocognitive outcomes in neurofibromatosis clinical trials: recommendations for the domain of attention. Neurology. 2016;87(5):S21–S30.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. 4.↵
    1. Posner MI,
    2. Petersen SE
    . The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25-42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. 5.↵
    1. Miyake A,
    2. Friedman NP,
    3. Emerson MJ,
    4. Witzki AH,
    5. Howerter A,
    6. Wager TD
    . The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49-100.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. 6.↵
    1. Wiebe SA,
    2. Espy KA,
    3. Charak D
    . Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children, I: latent structure. Dev Psych. 2008;44(2):575-587.
    OpenUrl
  7. 7.↵
    1. Stevens C,
    2. Bavelier D
    . The role of selective attention on academic foundations: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2012;2(suppl 1):S30–S48.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Diamantopoulou S,
    2. Rydell AM,
    3. Thorell LB,
    4. Bohlin G
    . Impact of executive functioning and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder on children's peer relations and school performance. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;32(1):521-542.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. 9.↵
    1. Gioia GA,
    2. Espy KA,
    3. Isquith PK
    . Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–Preschool Version: Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; 2003.
  10. 10.↵
    1. Reynolds C,
    2. Kamphaus R
    . BASC-2: Behavior Assessment System for Children. 2nd ed. American Guidance Service, Inc.; 2004.
  11. 11.↵
    1. Conners CK
    . The Conners Rating Scales–Revised Manual. Multi-Health Systems; 1997.
  12. 12.↵
    1. Achenbach T,
    2. Rescorla L
    . Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001.
  13. 13.↵
    1. Rietman AB,
    2. Oostenbrink R,
    3. Bongers S, et al
    . Motor problems in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. J Neurodevelop Disord. 2017;9:19.
    OpenUrl
  14. 14.↵
    1. Sangster J,
    2. Shores EA,
    3. Watt S,
    4. North KN
    . The cognitive profile of preschool-aged children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Child Neuropsychol. 2011;17(1):1-16.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  15. 15.↵
    1. Lorenzo J,
    2. Barton B,
    3. Acosta MT,
    4. North K
    . Mental, motor, and language development of toddlers with neurofibromatosis type 1. J Pediatr. 2011;158(4):660-665.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. 16.↵
    1. Lorenzo J,
    2. Barton B,
    3. Arnold SS,
    4. North KN
    . Cognitive features that distinguish preschool-age children with neurofibromatosis type 1 from their peers: a matched case-control study. J Pediatr. 2013;163(5):1479-1483.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. 17.↵
    1. Brei NG,
    2. Klein-Tasman BP,
    3. Schwarz GN,
    4. Casnar CL
    . Language in young children with neurofibromatosis-1: relations to functional communication, attention, and social functioning. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(10):2495-2504.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    1. Klein-Tasman BP,
    2. Janke KM,
    3. Luo W, et al
    . Cognitive and psychosocial phenotype of young children with neurofibromatosis-1. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):88-98.
    OpenUrl
  19. 19.↵
    1. Casnar CL,
    2. Janke KM,
    3. Fluit Fvander,
    4. Brei NG,
    5. Klein-Tasman BP
    . Relations between fine motor skill and parental report of attention in young children with neurofibromatosis type 1. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2014;36(9):930-943.
    OpenUrl
  20. 20.↵
    1. Casnar CL,
    2. Klein-Tasman BP
    . Parent and teacher perspectives on emerging executive functioning in preschoolers with neurofibromatosis type 1: comparison to unaffected children and lab-based measures. J Pediatr Psychol. 2016;42(2):198-207.
    OpenUrl
  21. 21.↵
    1. Espy KA
    . The Shape School: assessing executive function in preschool children. Dev Neuropsychol. 1997;13(4):495-499.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. 22.↵
    1. Conners CK
    . Conners' Kiddie Continuous Performance Test (K-CPT). Multi-Health Systems Inc; 2006.
  23. 23.↵
    1. Elliot CD
    . Differential Ability Scales–Second Edition: Administration and Scoring Manual. 2nd ed Harcourt Assessment; 2007.
  24. 24.↵
    1. Korkman M,
    2. Kirk U,
    3. Kemp S
    . NEPSY-II: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment. The Psychological Corp; 2007.
  25. 25.↵
    1. Welsh MC,
    2. Pennington BF,
    3. Groisser DB
    . A normative‐developmental study of executive function: a window on prefrontal function in children. Dev Neuropsychol. 1991;7:131-149.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  26. 26.↵
    1. Espy KA,
    2. Kaufmann PM,
    3. McDiarmid MD,
    4. Glisky ML
    . Executive functioning in preschool children: performance on A-not-B and other delayed response format tasks. Brain Cogn. 1999;41(2):178-199.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. 27.↵
    1. Rietman AB,
    2. Oostenbrink R,
    3. van Noort K, et al
    . Development of emotional and behavioral problems in neurofibromatosis type 1 during young childhood. Am J Med Genet. 2017;173A(9):2373-2380.
    OpenUrl
  28. 28.↵
    1. Tamm L,
    2. Epstein JN,
    3. Loren REA, et al
    . Generating attention, inhibition, and memory: a pilot randomized trial for preschoolers with executive functioning deficits. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol Routledge. 2019;48(sup 1):S131–S145.
    OpenUrl
  29. 29.↵
    1. Döpfner M,
    2. Dose C,
    3. Breuer D,
    4. Heintz S,
    5. Schiffhauer S,
    6. Banaschewski T
    . Efficacy of omega-3/omega-6 fatty acids in preschool children at risk of ADHD: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. J Atten Disord. 2021;25(8):1096-1106.
    OpenUrl
  30. 30.↵
    1. McGoey KE,
    2. DuPaul GJ,
    3. Haley E,
    4. Shelton TL
    . Parent and teacher ratings of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool: the ADHD Rating Scale-IV preschool version. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2007;29:269-276.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. 31.↵
    1. Jenkins JA,
    2. Lindskog CO
    1. DuPaul GJ,
    2. Power TJ,
    3. Anastopoulos AD,
    4. Reid R
    . ADHD Rating Scale–IV. Jenkins JA, Lindskog CO, editors. Accessed April 9, 2020. Available at: ezproxy.lib.uwm.edu/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct= true&AuthType=ip,uid&db=mmt&AN=test.2224&site=ehost-live&scope=site.
  32. 32.↵
    1. Tsai CJ,
    2. Chen YL,
    3. Lin HY,
    4. Gau SSF
    . One-year trajectory analysis for ADHD symptoms and its associated factors in community-based children and adolescents in Taiwan. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2017;11:28.
    OpenUrl
  33. 33.↵
    1. Ghuman JK,
    2. Aman MG,
    3. Lecavalier L, et al
    . Randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover study of methylphenidate for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in preschoolers with developmental disorders. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2009;19:329-339.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. 34.↵
    1. Abikoff HB,
    2. Thompson M,
    3. Laver‐Bradbury C, et al
    . Parent training for preschool ADHD: a randomized controlled trial of specialized and generic programs. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56:618-631.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. 35.↵
    1. Axelrad ME,
    2. Butler AM,
    3. Dempsey J,
    4. Chapman SG
    . Treatment effectiveness of a brief behavioral intervention for preschool disruptive behavior. J Clin Psychol Med Settings. 2013;20:323-332.
    OpenUrl
  36. 36.↵
    1. Eyberg S,
    2. Ross A
    . Assessment of child behavior problems: the validation of a new inventory. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 1978;7:113-116.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. 37.↵
    1. Childress AC,
    2. Kollins SH,
    3. Foehl HC, et al
    . Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, flexible-dose titration study of methylphenidate hydrochloride extended-release capsules (aptensio XR) in preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2019;30:58-68.
    OpenUrl
  38. 38.↵
    1. Conners CK
    . Conners Early Childhood: Manual. 3rd ed. Multi-Health Systems Inc; 2009.
  39. 39.↵
    1. Varni JW,
    2. Seid M,
    3. Kurtin PS
    . PedsQLTM 4.0: reliability and validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life InventoryTM version 4.0 Generic Core Scales in healthy and patient populations. Med Care. 2001;39:800-812.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. 40.↵
    1. Cohen SCL,
    2. Harvey DJ,
    3. Shields RH, et al
    . Effects of yoga on attention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity in preschool-aged children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2018;39:200-209.
    OpenUrl
  41. 41.↵
    1. Zimmermann P,
    2. Gondan M,
    3. Fimm B
    . KiTAP: Test for Attentional Performance in Children. Psytest; 2002.
  42. 42.↵
    1. Gischlar KL,
    2. Dombrowski S,
    3. Mahdavi JN
    1. Conners CK
    . Conners Continuous Performance Test 3rd Edition. Gischlar KL, Dombrowski S, Mahdavi JN, editors. Multi-Health Systems Inc; 2014.
  43. 43.↵
    1. Carlson SM,
    2. Zelazo PD
    . Minnesota Executive Function Scale: Test Manual. Reflection Sciences, LLC; 2014.
  44. 44.↵
    1. Flook L,
    2. Goldberg SB,
    3. Pinger L,
    4. Davidson RJ
    . Promoting prosocial behavior and self-regulatory skills in preschool children through a mindfulness-based kindness curriculum. Dev Psychol. 2015;51:44-51.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. 45.↵
    1. Arnold SS,
    2. Payne JM,
    3. Lorenzo J,
    4. North KN,
    5. Barton B
    . Preliteracy impairments in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2018;60:703-710.
    OpenUrl
  46. 46.↵
    1. Berger I,
    2. Slobodin O,
    3. Cassuto H
    . Usefulness and validity of continuous performance tests in the diagnosis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder children. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32:81-93.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. 47.↵
    1. Tamm L,
    2. Narad ME,
    3. Antonini TN,
    4. O'Brien KM,
    5. Hawk LW,
    6. Epstein JN
    . Reaction time variability in ADHD: a review. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9:500-508.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. 48.↵
    ADHD: clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics. 2011;128:1007-1022.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. 49.↵
    1. Garon N,
    2. Bryson S,
    3. Smith I
    . Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psych Bull. 2008;134:31-60.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. 50.↵
    1. Anderson PJ,
    2. Reidy N
    . Assessing executive function in preschoolers. Neuropsychol Rev. 2012;22:345-360.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. 51.↵
    1. Heffelfinger AK,
    2. Koop JI
    . A description of preschool neuropsychological assessment in the P.I.N.T. Clinic after the first 5 years. Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;23:51-76.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  52. 52.↵
    1. Payne JM,
    2. Hearps SJC,
    3. Walsh KS, et al
    . Reproducibility of cognitive endpoints in clinical trials: lessons from neurofibromatosis type 1. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2019;6:2555-2565.
    OpenUrl
  53. 53.
    1. Garg S,
    2. Lehtonen A,
    3. Huson SM, et al
    . Autism and other psychiatric comorbidity in neurofibromatosis type 1: evidence from a population‐based study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55:139-145.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. 54.
    1. Reynolds C,
    2. Kamphaus R
    . Behavior Assessment System for Children–Third Edition (BASC-3). Pearson; 2015.
  55. 55.
    1. Conners CK
    . Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2nd Edition (Conners K-CPT 2). Multi-Health Systems Inc; 2015.
  56. 56.
    1. Zelazo PD,
    2. Anderson JE,
    3. Richler J,
    4. Wallner-Allen K,
    5. Beaumont JL,
    6. Weintraub S
    . II. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring executive function and attention. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2013;78:16-33.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. 57.
    1. Tulsky DS,
    2. Carlozzi N,
    3. Chiaravalloti ND, et al
    . NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB): the list sorting test to measure working memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2015;20:599-610.
    OpenUrl
  58. 58.
    1. Cogstate
    . Cogstate Pediatric and Adult Normative Data. Cogstate Ltd.; 2018.
  59. 59.
    1. Greenberg LM,
    2. Waldmant ID
    . Developmental normative data on the test of variables of attention (T.O.V.A.TM). J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1993;34:1019-1030.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed

Letters: Rapid online correspondence

No comments have been published for this article.
Comment

REQUIREMENTS

You must ensure that your Disclosures have been updated within the previous six months. Please go to our Submission Site to add or update your Disclosure information.

Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.

If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.

Submission specifications:

  • Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
  • Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
  • Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
  • Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
  • Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.

More guidelines and information on Disputes & Debates

Compose Comment

More information about text formats

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
Author Information
NOTE: The first author must also be the corresponding author of the comment.
First or given name, e.g. 'Peter'.
Your last, or family, name, e.g. 'MacMoody'.
Your email address, e.g. higgs-boson@gmail.com
Your role and/or occupation, e.g. 'Orthopedic Surgeon'.
Your organization or institution (if applicable), e.g. 'Royal Free Hospital'.
Publishing Agreement
NOTE: All authors, besides the first/corresponding author, must complete a separate Publishing Agreement Form and provide via email to the editorial office before comments can be posted.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.

Vertical Tabs

You May Also be Interested in

Back to top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Research About Attention and Emerging Executive Functioning in Children With NF1
    • Clinical Trials Related to Attention in Preschoolers
    • Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Study Funding
    • Disclosure
    • Acknowledgment
    • Appendix Authors
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Advertisement

Cutaneous α-Synuclein Signatures in Patients With Multiple System Atrophy and Parkinson Disease

Dr. Rizwan S. Akhtar and Dr. Sarah Brooker

► Watch

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.

Alert Me

  • Alert me when eletters are published

Recommended articles

  • Article
    Neurocognitive outcomes in neurofibromatosis clinical trials
    Recommendations for the domain of attention
    Karin S. Walsh, Jennifer Janusz, Pamela L. Wolters et al.
    Neurology, August 15, 2016
  • Patient Reported Outcomes
    Recommendations for Social Skills End Points for Clinical Trials in Neurofibromatosis Type 1
    Jennifer A. Janusz, Bonita P. Klein-Tasman, Jonathan M. Payne et al.
    Neurology, July 06, 2021
  • Patient Reported Outcomes
    Current Recommendations for Patient-Reported Outcome Measures Assessing Domains of Quality of Life in Neurofibromatosis Clinical Trials
    Pamela L. Wolters, Ana-Maria Vranceanu, Heather L. Thompson et al.
    Neurology, July 06, 2021
  • Patient Reported Outcomes
    Patient Report of Hearing in Neurofibromatosis Type 2
    Recommendations for Clinical Trials
    Heather L. Thompson, Ann Blanton, Barbara Franklin et al.
    Neurology, July 06, 2021
Neurology: 100 (22)

Articles

  • Ahead of Print
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Popular Articles
  • Translations

About

  • About the Journals
  • Ethics Policies
  • Editors & Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertise

Submit

  • Author Center
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Information for Reviewers
  • AAN Guidelines
  • Permissions

Subscribers

  • Subscribe
  • Activate a Subscription
  • Sign up for eAlerts
  • RSS Feed
Site Logo
  • Visit neurology Template on Facebook
  • Follow neurology Template on Twitter
  • Visit Neurology on YouTube
  • Neurology
  • Neurology: Clinical Practice
  • Neurology: Education
  • Neurology: Genetics
  • Neurology: Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • AAN.com
  • AANnews
  • Continuum
  • Brain & Life
  • Neurology Today

Wolters Kluwer Logo

Neurology | Print ISSN:0028-3878
Online ISSN:1526-632X

© 2023 American Academy of Neurology

  • Privacy Policy
  • Feedback
  • Advertise