Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion (DEI)
    • Neurology: Clinical Practice Accelerator
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit New Manuscript
    • Submit Revised Manuscript
    • Author Center

Advanced Search

Main menu

  • Neurology.org
  • Journals
    • Neurology
    • Clinical Practice
    • Education
    • Genetics
    • Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • Online Sections
    • Neurology Video Journal Club
    • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion (DEI)
    • Neurology: Clinical Practice Accelerator
    • Practice Buzz
    • Practice Current
    • Residents & Fellows
    • Without Borders
  • Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Disputes & Debates
    • Health Disparities
    • Infographics
    • Neurology Future Forecasting Series
    • Null Hypothesis
    • Patient Pages
    • Topics A-Z
    • Translations
  • Podcast
  • CME
  • About
    • About the Journals
    • Contact Us
    • Editorial Board
  • Authors
    • Submit New Manuscript
    • Submit Revised Manuscript
    • Author Center
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Neurology
Home
The most widely read and highly cited peer-reviewed neurology journal
  • Subscribe
  • My Alerts
  • Log in
Site Logo
  • Home
  • Latest Articles
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Neurology Video Journal Club
  • Residents & Fellows

Share

February 15, 2022; 98 (7) Research ArticleOpen Access

Effect of Age at Pediatric Stroke on Long-term Cognitive Outcome

Stephanie Abgottspon, Qendresa Thaqi, Leonie Steiner, Nedelina Slavova, Sebastian Grunt, Maja Steinlin, Regula Everts
First published December 16, 2021, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000013207
Stephanie Abgottspon
From the Division of Neuropediatrics, Development and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics (S.A., Q.T., L.S., S.G., M.S., R.E.), Department of Diabetes, Endocrinology, Nutritional Medicine and Metabolism (S.A., R.E.), and Institute of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology (N.S.), Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern; Graduate School for Health Sciences (S.A., L.S.) and Department of Psychology (Q.T.), University of Bern; and Pediatric Radiology (N.S.), University Children's Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Qendresa Thaqi
From the Division of Neuropediatrics, Development and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics (S.A., Q.T., L.S., S.G., M.S., R.E.), Department of Diabetes, Endocrinology, Nutritional Medicine and Metabolism (S.A., R.E.), and Institute of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology (N.S.), Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern; Graduate School for Health Sciences (S.A., L.S.) and Department of Psychology (Q.T.), University of Bern; and Pediatric Radiology (N.S.), University Children's Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Leonie Steiner
From the Division of Neuropediatrics, Development and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics (S.A., Q.T., L.S., S.G., M.S., R.E.), Department of Diabetes, Endocrinology, Nutritional Medicine and Metabolism (S.A., R.E.), and Institute of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology (N.S.), Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern; Graduate School for Health Sciences (S.A., L.S.) and Department of Psychology (Q.T.), University of Bern; and Pediatric Radiology (N.S.), University Children's Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nedelina Slavova
From the Division of Neuropediatrics, Development and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics (S.A., Q.T., L.S., S.G., M.S., R.E.), Department of Diabetes, Endocrinology, Nutritional Medicine and Metabolism (S.A., R.E.), and Institute of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology (N.S.), Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern; Graduate School for Health Sciences (S.A., L.S.) and Department of Psychology (Q.T.), University of Bern; and Pediatric Radiology (N.S.), University Children's Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sebastian Grunt
From the Division of Neuropediatrics, Development and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics (S.A., Q.T., L.S., S.G., M.S., R.E.), Department of Diabetes, Endocrinology, Nutritional Medicine and Metabolism (S.A., R.E.), and Institute of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology (N.S.), Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern; Graduate School for Health Sciences (S.A., L.S.) and Department of Psychology (Q.T.), University of Bern; and Pediatric Radiology (N.S.), University Children's Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Maja Steinlin
From the Division of Neuropediatrics, Development and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics (S.A., Q.T., L.S., S.G., M.S., R.E.), Department of Diabetes, Endocrinology, Nutritional Medicine and Metabolism (S.A., R.E.), and Institute of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology (N.S.), Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern; Graduate School for Health Sciences (S.A., L.S.) and Department of Psychology (Q.T.), University of Bern; and Pediatric Radiology (N.S.), University Children's Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Regula Everts
From the Division of Neuropediatrics, Development and Rehabilitation, Department of Pediatrics (S.A., Q.T., L.S., S.G., M.S., R.E.), Department of Diabetes, Endocrinology, Nutritional Medicine and Metabolism (S.A., R.E.), and Institute of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology (N.S.), Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern; Graduate School for Health Sciences (S.A., L.S.) and Department of Psychology (Q.T.), University of Bern; and Pediatric Radiology (N.S.), University Children's Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Switzerland.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Full PDF
Short Form
Citation
Effect of Age at Pediatric Stroke on Long-term Cognitive Outcome
Stephanie Abgottspon, Qendresa Thaqi, Leonie Steiner, Nedelina Slavova, Sebastian Grunt, Maja Steinlin, Regula Everts
Neurology Feb 2022, 98 (7) e721-e729; DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000013207

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Permissions

Make Comment

See Comments

Downloads
1511

Share

  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Loading

Abstract

Background and Objectives To investigate the effect of age at pediatric arterial ischemic stroke on long-term cognitive outcome in order to identify patients particularly at risk for the development of long-term cognitive sequelae.

Methods This cross-sectional study included patients in the chronic phase of stroke (>2 years after stroke) previously diagnosed with neonatal or childhood arterial ischemic stroke and a control group. Participants with active epilepsy, severe learning difficulties, or behavioral problems hindering the cognitive assessment were excluded. Several cognitive domains, including intelligence, executive functions (working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility), processing speed, memory, letter fluency, and visual-motor skills were assessed with neuropsychological tests. Cognitive long-term outcome was compared across patients after neonatal stroke (stroke between 0 and 28 days of life), early childhood stroke (stroke between 29 days and <6 years), and late childhood stroke (stroke between ≥6 and <16 years).

Results Fifty-two patients after neonatal or childhood arterial ischemic stroke (median age 15.3 years, interquartile range [IQR] 10.6–18.7) and 49 healthy controls (median age 13.6 years, IQR 9.8–17.2) met the inclusion criteria. Cognitive outcome was significantly worse in the pediatric stroke group compared to the control group. A nonlinear effect of age at stroke (irrespective of lesion size and lesion location) was found for cognitive flexibility, processing speed, and verbal learning with early childhood stroke (29 days to <6 years), showing significantly worse cognitive outcome compared to neonatal or late childhood stroke (p < 0.05, false discovery rate–corrected).

Discussion Age at stroke is an important factor for poststroke recovery and modulates long-term cognitive outcome irrespective of lesion size and lesion location. Children after early childhood stroke are at particular risk for long-term alterations in cognitive functions.

Glossary

ANCOVA=
analysis of covariance;
ANOVA=
analysis of variance;
FDR=
false discovery rate;
HERO=
Hemispheric Reorganization study;
pedASPECTS=
pediatric modification of the Alberta Stroke Program Early Computed Tomography Score;
PSOM=
Pediatric Stroke Outcome Measure;
SNPSR=
Swiss Neuropediatric Stroke Registry

Pediatric arterial ischemic stroke is a rare event accompanied by an increased risk for cognitive and neurologic sequelae.1,-,3 Identification of factors associated with poor outcome has been the focus of research for several years, but knowledge about outcome prediction remains insufficient.4,-,6

An inherent property of the developing brain is increased plasticity.7,-,9 Due to rapid synaptogenesis as well as increased myelination and reorganization processes of neuronal networks during this period, the developing brain is suggested to be more flexible with better recovery capacity after early brain insult.2,7,9 On the other hand, the developing brain is particularly vulnerable to early brain insult, leading to disrupted brain development.7,9 In light of the 2 contradictory perspectives—plasticity vs vulnerability—findings about the effect of age at pediatric stroke remain unclear, with some studies showing that younger age at stroke is associated with worse6,10,-,13 or better cognitive outcome.14,15

Whereas several studies examined cognitive outcome in the acute phase of the stroke up until 2 years poststroke,6,10,12 studies investigating the outcome of patients in the late chronic stage (>2 years after stroke) are limited. Focusing on patients in the late chronic stage is essential as deficits may emerge and increase over time5,11,16 and recovery processes can extend far beyond the first months poststroke.17 Furthermore, the developmental stage at the time of the brain insult can modulate cognitive outcome.18,19 Brain insults during a critical period of cognitive development likely entail poorer cognitive outcome compared to brain injuries occurring before, during, or after the emergence of cognitive function.19 Moreover, cognitive functions, in particular executive functions, are strongly associated with quality of life,20 scholastic achievement,21 and social competence,22 which highlights the importance to monitor poststroke cognitive outcome.

In the present study, we aimed to examine long-term cognitive outcome following pediatric stroke in the chronic stage (>2 years after stroke) and to investigate whether age at stroke affects cognition in order to identify patients at risk for poor cognitive outcome.

Methods

Study Design and Study Population

This cross-sectional study includes data from 2 research projects that were carried out at the Division of Neuropediatrics, Development, and Rehabilitation at the University Hospital in Berne, Switzerland (Hemispheric Reorganization3 [HERO] study,3 2014–2016, and Onset study, 2019–2020). In both research projects, participants diagnosed with pediatric arterial ischemic stroke were recruited from the population-based Swiss Neuropediatric Stroke Registry (SNPSR).6,23 The control group was recruited within the HERO study through advertisement in the hospital intranet and flyers.

Inclusion criteria for the stroke group were a diagnosis of pediatric arterial ischemic stroke (neonatal or childhood stroke, confirmed by MRI or CT) at least 2 years prior to study participation, age at stroke ≤16 years, and age at examination ≥6 years. Exclusion criteria were active epilepsy (defined as seizures or treatment with antiseizure medication during the 12 months prior to study participation), additional neurologic disorders not attributable to stroke, severe learning difficulties, or pronounced behavioral problems that would make assessments impossible. Patients with active epilepsy were excluded because transcranial magnetic stimulation was performed as part of the HERO study. Inclusion criteria for the control group were age at examination ≥6 years and no impairments influencing cognitive and neurologic development. Exclusion criteria were the same as for the stroke sample.

Standard Protocol Approvals, Registrations, and Patient Consents

The study protocols of both research projects were approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Berne, Switzerland (HERO study: 212/13,3 Onset study: 2019–00546). Depending on the age of the participants, written informed consent was obtained from the participant (if >16 years of age) or parent/legal guardian (if <16 years of age). Examinations were performed at the University Hospital, Inselspital Bern, or, in individual cases, home visits were performed. Both studies were conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

Clinical Data and Lesion Characteristics

Clinical data, including sex, age at stroke, and stroke risk factors, were obtained from medical records and the SNPSR database. Stroke risk factors were categorized according to Steinlin and Wehrli.24 Lesion volume was estimated using the pediatric modification of the Alberta Stroke Program Early Computed Tomography Score (pedASPECTS),25,-,27 a score that was validated in a previous study in 71 individuals with neonatal or childhood arterial ischemic stroke.25 The procedure is described elsewhere.28,29 Maximum score of the pedASPECTS is 30, indicating uttermost severity. Scores were determined based on diffusion-weighted imaging sequences from the acute MRI, or—if not available—on morphologic magnetic resonance sequences of the first-available poststroke brain MRI (T2-weighted imaging or fluid-attenuated inversion recovery imaging). For 7 patients, the acute MRI was not available and hence the pedASPECTS was determined using the first available poststroke MRI. PedASPECTS of patients with poststroke MRI did not differ from pedASPECTS determined on the acute MRI (p = 0.282). Information about lesion laterality (left, right, or bilateral) and lesion location (cortical, subcortical, or combined cortical and subcortical) were derived from the pedASPECTS.30 Neurologic outcome at the time of the study assessment was assessed via the Pediatric Stroke Outcome Measure (PSOM).31 The PSOM consists of 5 subscales (right sensorimotor, left sensorimotor, language production, language comprehension, and cognitive/behavior) and yields a total score ranging from 0 (no deficits) to 10 (maximum deficits).

Cognitive Assessments

Several cognitive domains, such as nonverbal intelligence, executive functions, processing speed, memory, letter fluency, and visual-motor skills, were evaluated during the neuropsychological assessment by an experienced neuropsychologist. A description of the cognitive tests and the outcomes is provided in Table 1. For all tests, age-corrected standard scores, index scores, or percentile ranks were used according to the test manual.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1

Overview of the Cognitive Assessment

Data Analysis

To assess the effect of stroke on long-term cognitive outcome, the stroke sample was divided into 3 age at stroke groups: neonatal stroke (stroke between 0 and 28 days of life), early childhood stroke (stroke between 29 days and <6 years), and late childhood stroke (stroke between ≥6 and <16 years) such as described in a previous study.11 We performed locally weighted regression and scatterplot smoothing (LOESS) to display possible associations between cognitive outcome and age at stroke and to verify the cutoff points of the age at stroke groups.32

Demographic and clinical data were presented for the control group and the 3 age at stroke groups. Continuous variables were reported as mean and SD for normally distributed variables and median and interquartile range for non-normally distributed variables. Categorical variables were presented as frequencies and percentages. Descriptive and baseline variables in the 3 age at stroke groups were compared using analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with Bonferroni post hoc tests (or Kruskal-Wallis tests for nonparametric data) and Pearson χ2 tests followed by post hoc pairwise comparisons. Group differences in cognitive performance between the total stroke sample and the control sample were computed using 2-sided independent t tests. To identify possible confounders, associations between lesion size, lesion location, lesion laterality, and cognitive outcome (within the total stroke group) were examined with 2-sided Spearman correlations or ANOVAs, respectively. We did not investigate associations between stroke risk factors and cognitive outcome because of the limited number of patients in each stroke risk factor category.

To investigate the effect of age at stroke on long-term cognitive outcome, a series of one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) using the 3 age at stroke groups as independent variables and the respective cognitive domain as dependent variable (controlling for the effect of lesion size and lesion location) were conducted followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests. Results of the ANCOVA analyses were reported as estimated marginal means with associated 95% confidence intervals. Cohen d effect sizes were calculated for independent t test and partial eta squared (ηp2) for ANCOVAs.33 Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. We report alpha values adjusted according to the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure (false discovery rate [FDR]) to correct for multiple testing.34 All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS, version 25. Data visualization was generated using the R package ggplot2.35

Data Availability

All anonymized data are available on request from any qualified investigator.

Results

Baseline Characteristics

Fifty-four participants diagnosed with pediatric stroke and 50 healthy controls were identified from the HERO or Onset study, of whom 52 patients and 49 healthy controls were included in the present study. One patient and one control did not meet the inclusion criteria for the present study because they were younger than 6 years and one patient was excluded due to missing data. The study flow chart is presented in eFigure 1, links.lww.com/WNL/B701. Demographic and clinical data are described in Table 2. Both groups were comparable in terms of age at examination (p = 0.587) and sex (p = 0.275). As an inherent characteristic of our subgroups, the 3 age at stroke groups differed significantly in their age at stroke (p < 0.000) and age at examination (p < 0.000). We did not adjust for the effect of age at examination in the following analysis, as we used age-corrected scores for all cognitive tests.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2

Demographic and Clinical Data for the Total Stroke Sample, the 3 Age at Stroke Groups, and the Control Sample

The 3 age at stroke groups were comparable in regard to sex, lesion size, and lesion laterality (Table 2). Neurologic outcome (PSOM) did not differ in the 3 age at stroke groups. A significant group effect was found for lesion location (p = 0.016), revealing that cortical lesions occurred significantly more often in the neonatal group (46.7%) compared to the late childhood stroke group (6.7%). Across the total stroke sample, lesion size correlated negatively with intelligence (r = −0.338, p = 0.016), working memory (r = −0.335, p = 0.017), inhibition (r = −0.381, p = 0.008), processing speed (r = −0.319, p = 0.024), letter fluency (r = −0.444, p = 0.002), and visual motor skills (r = −0.381, p = 0.050). Lesion location was not associated with cognitive outcome. Lesion laterality did not affect cognitive performance, except for cognitive flexibility (F = 4.198, p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.167), where right hemispheric lesions entailed worse performance than left hemispheric lesions (p = 0.018). As a consequence of these findings, all following ANCOVA analyses were adjusted for the effect of lesion size and lesion location.

Long-term Cognitive Outcome in Patients and Controls

Cognitive outcome in patients after pediatric stroke and healthy controls is displayed in Table 3. Although mean group performance was within the normative reference range for both groups, significantly lower cognitive performance occurred in all cognitive domains in patients vs controls (medium to large effect sizes). All group differences remained significant after adjusting for multiple comparisons (FDR correction).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3

Mean Cognitive Performance for the Total Stroke Sample, the 3 Age at Stroke Groups, and the Control Sample

Effect of Age at Stroke on Long-term Cognitive Outcome

LOESS plots are displayed in Figure 1. A nonlinear relationship was observed between age at stroke and cognitive outcome. Next, we investigated the effect of age at stroke on long-term cognitive outcome using a series of ANCOVAs controlling for the effect of lesion size and lesion location. Results are presented in Figure 2. A significant effect for age at stroke was found for performance in working memory (F = 4.131, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.155), cognitive flexibility (F = 5.368, p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.212), processing speed (F = 6.537, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.225), and verbal learning (F = 5.099, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.185) with early childhood stroke displaying the worst outcome. All effect sizes were interpreted as large. The significant effect of age at stroke on cognitive flexibility (p = 0.040), processing speed (p = 0.027), and verbal learning (p = 0.030) persisted after FDR correction, except for working memory (p = 0.052). Although not significant, intelligence (F = 2.793, p = 0.072, ηp2 = 0.110), inhibition (F = 1.137, p = 0.331, ηp2 = 0.051), verbal recall (F = 1.678, p = 0.199, ηp2 = 0.072), letter fluency (F = 1.748, p = 0.186, ηp2 = 0.077), and visual-motor skills (F = 3.141, p = 0.090, ηp2 = 0.120) were slightly lower in the early childhood stroke group compared to the neonatal and late childhood stroke group.

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1 Association Between Age at Stroke and Long-term Cognitive Outcome

LOESS plots of cognitive outcome as a function of age at stroke. X-axis represents age at stroke (in years). 95% confidence interval in gray. IS = index score; PR = percentile range; SS = standard score.

Figure 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2 Effect of Age at Stroke on Long-term Cognitive Outcome

A significant effect of age at stroke on cognitive flexibility, processing speed, and verbal learning was found (p < 0.05, false discovery rate–corrected). Estimated marginal means (adjusted for the mean of covariates) with associated 95% confidence interval are displayed on the X-axis for the 3 age at stroke groups: neonatal stroke (red), early childhood (green), and late childhood (blue). Due to missing normative data, data about visual-motor skills were not available for the late childhood stroke group. IS = index score; PR = percentile range; SS = standard score. Significant Bonferroni post hoc results: *p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

Discussion

In this cross-sectional study, we demonstrated that patients after pediatric stroke displayed worse cognitive performance compared to a control group and that age at stroke affects cognitive outcome. Neonatal stroke and late childhood stroke were associated with better outcome whereas early childhood stroke led to significantly worse outcome in cognitive flexibility, processing speed, and verbal learning, irrespective of lesion size and lesion location.

Our results suggest that age at stroke is an important factor for poststroke recovery and modulates long-term cognitive outcome even when controlling for lesion size and lesion location. In contrast to previous studies indicating that younger age at stroke relates to worse6,10,-,13 or better cognitive outcome,14,15 our data revealed a U-shaped association between age at stroke and long-term cognitive outcome, with children after early childhood stroke showing the worst outcome. Only a limited number of studies have shown a nonlinear effect of age at stroke on cognitive outcome.5,30,36 A study of 21 Swiss children after pediatric stroke found better cognitive performance in children who had a stroke at the ages between 5 and 10 years compared to earlier (0–5 years) or later (10–18 years).30 Similarly, Allman and Scott36 found in a sample of 44 participants that stroke occurring between the ages of 1 and 6 years entailed better cognitive outcome than earlier stroke (before the age of 1) and later stroke (6–16 years). Both studies demonstrated an inverted U-shaped association between age at stroke and cognitive outcome, which is exactly the opposite pattern compared to our data. In terms of neurologic outcome, a recent study with 587 patients after pediatric stroke concluded that younger children (28 days and 1 year at the time of the stroke) are particularly vulnerable for poor neurologic outcome (PSOM total score) 2 years after stroke when compared to children after neonatal stroke or stroke >1 year of age.5 This U-shaped relationship between age and outcome is in line with our results, but the PSOM assesses neurologic (i.e., sensorimotor, language, and cognitive functions) and not purely cognitive performance.

The current study included only patients in the chronic phase of the stroke (>2 years after stroke), whereas previous studies included patients with shorter follow-up periods as well.30,36 In fact, cognitive alterations may emerge and increase over time16 and the full extent of cognitive sequelae may only appear several years poststroke.7 Also, developmental processes and recovery trajectories may be different depending on the cognitive function measured.7 For instance, executive functions are not fully developed until early adulthood and rely on intact frontal and prefrontal cortices.37 This highlights the importance of long-term follow-ups in patients after pediatric stroke in order to identify patients particularly at risk for alterations in long-term cognitive outcome.7

There is an ongoing debate around plasticity and vulnerability of the developing brain following early brain injury.7,-,9 The early plasticity approach supports the idea of maximum plasticity in the developing brain with better recovery of cognitive functions after brain lesion in childhood compared to adulthood.7,8 In contrast, the vulnerability perspective argues that the developing brain is particularly vulnerable to stroke, which leads to disrupted brain and cognitive development.7,30,38 Our results did not favor one or the other perspective but support a recent idea combining these to concepts to a “recovery continuum,” suggesting that cognitive outcome after stroke is determined by several factors such as age at stroke, lesion-related characteristics, and sociodemographic factors.7 Hence, our results may be inconsistent with previous studies,30,36 because several factors affect cognitive outcome and, for instance, patient or lesion characteristics vary across studies.

Findings from the current study propose that early childhood is a particularly vulnerable developmental period in terms of poststroke cognitive outcome. Between the age of 29 days to <6 years, cognitive functions measured in our study are about to emerge and continuously develop; however, none of the cognitive domains we have measured is fully established yet. Hence, in line with previous findings from epilepsy research, we suggest that stroke during a critical period of cognitive development has a particularly detrimental effect on outcome.18,19 This is further supported by neuroimaging studies, revealing that functional and structural brain development is a nonlinear process with critical periods for plasticity as well as maturational processes (i.e., myelination and synaptogenesis).39,-,41

In addition, we examined our total stroke sample with regard to the effect of lesion size, lesion location, and lesion laterality on cognitive outcome. Larger lesion size was related to poorer performance in intelligence, working memory, inhibition, processing speed, letter fluency, and visual-motor skills in the present study, which is consistent with previous findings.30,42 Also in line with previous findings,11 we found that cortical lesions occurred more often in the neonatal group compared to the late childhood stroke group. In terms of the effect of lesion location on cognitive outcome, previous findings are inconsistent. Whereas Westmacott et al.11 suggest that combined lesions (cortical and subcortical) were associated with worse cognitive performance, other studies12,30 as well as our data did not reveal an effect of lesion location on cognitive outcome. However, different approaches to classify lesion location hinders comparability between studies. Furthermore, except for cognitive flexibility, lesion laterality did not affect cognitive performance. Our data support the functional network approach, claiming that even remote lesion locations can affect functional brain networks and thus affect cognitive performance.43

A strength of our study is the large sample size of 52 patients after pediatric stroke, a rare neurologic disease in childhood (incidence of neonatal ischemic stroke in Switzerland is 13 per 100,000 live births,44 incidence of childhood stroke in Switzerland is 2.1:100,000 children per year23). The inclusion of a control group further strengthens our study. We only included patients in the chronic phase of the stroke (>2 years after stroke), allowing us to draw conclusions about long-term cognitive outcome. Other strengths include the broad spectrum of cognitive functions assessed in this study. Furthermore, we estimated lesion size using a relatively new method in pediatric and pediatric neuroimaging, the pedASPECTS, which has fair to good accuracy for predicting cerebral palsy as well as neurologic impairment and was shown to correlate with lesion size.28 The findings remain robust after adjusting for multiple testing (FDR correction).

Some limitations have to be acknowledged. First, the cross-sectional design of this study does not allow us to draw conclusions about developmental trajectories of cognitive functions. Second, our age at stroke groups were defined according to a prior study.11 Other studies adopted different classification approaches.6,30,36 However, when defining the cutoffs differently, the effect of our results remained similar. Third, pediatric stroke is a heterogeneous disease with diverse clinical presentation and etiology, hindering comparability across studies. Fourth, for a small number of patients, acute neuroimaging was not available. Determining pedASPECTS on postacute imaging may have led to underestimation of the score because punctate lesions may become unrecognizable due to so-called pseudonormalization of diffusion restriction, or—in the chronic stage—may shrink over time and remain hardly visible. Yet pedASPECTS of patients with poststroke MRI did not differ from pedASPECTS determined on the acute MRI. Fifth, the patient group may be biased as it does not account for patients who were lost to follow-up or died during the poststroke phase. Likewise, a large number of potential participants were too young (n = 112) or unwilling to participate in the study (n = 191), increasing the risk of a potential selection bias.

We have shown that long-term cognitive outcome varied according to age at stroke in a population of pediatric stroke survivors without severe learning difficulties. Our findings suggest a nonlinear relationship between age at stroke and cognitive outcome with patients after early childhood stroke (29 days to <6 years) performing worse than patients after neonatal or late childhood stroke. These results propose that early childhood is a particularly vulnerable developmental period for negative long-term cognitive outcome in survivors of pediatric stroke, irrespective of lesion size and lesion location. Children with early childhood stroke should be monitored closely and provided with adequate treatment and rehabilitation options tailored on their age at stroke and current developmental period in order to prevent cognitive sequelae and improve cognitive outcome.

Study Funding

This study was supported by the Jacobs Foundation, the Stiftung Batzebär of the Children's University Hospital Bern (Switzerland), the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNF grant 32003B_146,894/1), the Schweizerische Stiftung für das Cerebral Gelähmte Kind (Switzerland), the Vinetum foundation (Switzerland), and the Anna Müller Grocholski Foundation (Switzerland).

Disclosure

The authors report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures.

Acknowledgment

The authors thank the children, adolescents, and young adults who participated in the studies, as well as their parents; Salome Kornfeld, Juan Delgado-Rodriguez, Sandeep Kamal, Maria Regényi, and Jasmin Grolimund for their support in study coordination and data collection; Sarah Tanner for her help with the literature search; and the coworkers at the Swiss Neuropediatric Stroke Registry: Andrea Capone Mori (Aarau), Alexandre Datta (Basel), Joël Fluss (Geneva), Annette Hackenberg (Zürich), Elmar Keller (Chur), Oliver Maier (St. Gallen), Jean-Pierre Marcoz (Sion), Claudia Poloni (Lausanne), Gian Paolo Ramelli (Bellinzona), Regula Schmid (Winterthur), and Thomas Schmitt-Mechelke (Lucerne).

Appendix Authors

Table

Footnotes

  • Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article.

  • The article processing charge was paid for by the Swiss National Science Foundation.

  • Editorial, page 263

  • Received May 31, 2021.
  • Accepted in final form November 23, 2021.
  • Copyright © 2021 The Author(s). Published by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. on behalf of the American Academy of Neurology.

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND), which permits downloading and sharing the work provided it is properly cited. The work cannot be changed in any way or used commercially without permission from the journal.

References

  1. 1.↵
    1. Steinlin M
    . A clinical approach to arterial ischemic childhood stroke: increasing knowledge over the last decade. Neuropediatrics. 2012;43(1):1-9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. 2.↵
    1. Jordan LC,
    2. Hillis AE
    . Challenges in the diagnosis and treatment of pediatric stroke. Nat Rev Neurol. 2011;7(4):199-208.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. 3.↵
    1. Kornfeld S,
    2. Delgado Rodríguez JA,
    3. Everts R, et al
    . Cortical reorganisation of cerebral networks after childhood stroke: impact on outcome. BMC Neurol. 2015;15(1):90.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. 4.↵
    1. Jiang B,
    2. Hills NK,
    3. Forsyth R, et al.
    Imaging predictors of neurologic outcome after pediatric arterial ischemic stroke. Stroke. 2020:152-161.
  5. 5.↵
    1. Felling RJ,
    2. Rafay MF,
    3. Bernard TJ, et al.
    Predicting recovery and outcome after pediatric stroke: results from the international pediatric stroke study. Ann Neurol. 2020;87(6):840-852.
    OpenUrl
  6. 6.↵
    1. Studer M,
    2. Boltshauser E,
    3. Capone Mori A, et al.
    Factors affecting cognitive outcome in early pediatric stroke. Neurology. 2014;82:784-792.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. 7.↵
    1. Anderson V,
    2. Spencer-Smith M,
    3. Wood A
    . Do children really recover better? Neurobehavioural plasticity after early brain insult. Brain. 2011;134:2197-2221.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Ballantyne AO,
    2. Spilkin AM,
    3. Hesselink J,
    4. Trauner DA
    . Plasticity in the developing brain: intellectual, language and academic functions in children with ischaemic perinatal stroke. Brain. 2008;131(11):2975-2985.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. 9.↵
    1. Johnston MV
    . Plasticity in the developing brain: implications for rehabilitation. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2009;15(2):94-101.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. 10.↵
    1. Jacomb I,
    2. Porter M,
    3. Brunsdon R,
    4. Mandalis A,
    5. Parry L
    . Cognitive outcomes of pediatric stroke. Child Neuropsychol. 2018;24(3):287-303.
    OpenUrl
  11. 11.↵
    1. Westmacott R,
    2. Askalan R,
    3. Macgregor D,
    4. Anderson P,
    5. Deveber G
    . Cognitive outcome following unilateral arterial ischaemic stroke in childhood: effects of age at stroke and lesion location. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52(4):386-393.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. 12.↵
    1. Pavlovic J,
    2. Kaufmann F,
    3. Boltshauser E, et al.
    Neuropsychological problems after paediatric stroke: two year follow-up of Swiss children. Neuropediatrics. 2006;37:13-19.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. 13.↵
    1. Lansing AE,
    2. Max JE,
    3. Delis DC, et al.
    Verbal learning and memory after childhood stroke. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(5):742-752.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. 14.↵
    1. O'Keeffe F,
    2. Liégeois F,
    3. Eve M,
    4. Ganesan V,
    5. King J,
    6. Murphy T
    . Neuropsychological and neurobehavioral outcome following childhood arterial ischemic stroke: attention deficits, emotional dysregulation, and executive dysfunction. Child Neuropsychol. 2014;20(5):557-582.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. 15.↵
    1. DeVeber GA,
    2. MacGregor D,
    3. Curtis R,
    4. Mayank S
    . Neurologic outcome in survivors of childhood arterial ischemic stroke and sinovenous thrombosis. J Child Neurol. 2000;15(5):316-324.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. 16.↵
    1. Westmacott R,
    2. Macgregor D,
    3. Askalan R,
    4. Deveber G
    . Late emergence of cognitive deficits after unilateral neonatal stroke. Stroke. 2009;40(6):2012-2019.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. 17.↵
    1. Ballester BR,
    2. Maier M,
    3. Duff A, et al.
    A critical time window for recovery extends beyond one-year post-stroke. J Neurophysiol. 2019;122(1):350-357.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    1. Fox SE,
    2. Levitt P,
    3. Nelson CA
    . How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Dev. 2010;81(1):28-40.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. 19.↵
    1. Gonzalez LM,
    2. Embuldeniya US,
    3. Harvey AS, et al.
    Developmental stage affects cognition in children with recently-diagnosed symptomatic focal epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2014;39:97-104.
    OpenUrl
  20. 20.↵
    1. Laporta-Hoyos O,
    2. Ballester-Plané J,
    3. Póo P, et al.
    Proxy-reported quality of life in adolescents and adults with dyskinetic cerebral palsy is associated with executive functions and cortical thickness. Qual Life Res. 2017;26(5):1209-1222.
    OpenUrl
  21. 21.↵
    1. St Clair-Thompson HL,
    2. Gathercole SE
    . Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 2006;59(4):745-759.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. 22.↵
    1. Semrud-Clikeman M
    . Social Competence in Children. Springer; 2007.
  23. 23.↵
    1. Steinlin M,
    2. Pfister I,
    3. Pavlovic J, et al.
    The first three years of the Swiss neuropaediatric stroke Registry (SNPSR): a population-based study of incidence, symptoms and risk factors. Neuropediatrics. 2005;36(2):90-97.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. 24.↵
    1. Steinlin M,
    2. Wehrli E
    . Der kindliche ischämische Schlaganfall. Wann daran denken–was unternehmen? Paediatrica. 2009;20(2):17-21.
    OpenUrl
  25. 25.↵
    1. Beslow LA,
    2. Vossough A,
    3. Dahmoush HM, et al.
    Modified pediatric ASPECTS correlates with infarct volume in childhood arterial ischemic stroke. Front Neurol. Epub 2012 Jul 31.
  26. 26.↵
    1. Barber PA,
    2. Demchuk AM,
    3. Zhang J,
    4. Buchan AM,
    5. Study A
    . Validity and reliability of a quantitative computed tomography score in predicting outcome of hyperacute stroke before thrombolytic therapy. Lancet. 2000;355:1670-1674.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. 27.↵
    1. Schröder J,
    2. Thomalla G
    . A critical review of Alberta Stroke Program Early CT Score for evaluation of acute stroke imaging. Front Neurol. 2017;7.
  28. 28.↵
    1. Mackay MT,
    2. Slavova N,
    3. Pastore-Wapp M, et al.
    Pediatric ASPECTS predicts outcomes following acute symptomatic neonatal arterial stroke. Neurology. 2020;94(12):e1259–e1270.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. 29.↵
    1. Slavova N,
    2. Fullerton HJ,
    3. Hills NK,
    4. Breiding PS,
    5. Mackay MT,
    6. Steinlin M
    . Validation of the Focal Cerebral Arteriopathy Severity Score (FCASS) in a Swiss cohort: correlation with infarct volume and outcome. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2020;28:58-63.
    OpenUrl
  30. 30.↵
    1. Everts R,
    2. Pavlovic J,
    3. Kaufmann F, et al.
    Cognitive functioning, behavior, and quality of life after stroke in childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2008;14(4):323-338.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. 31.↵
    1. Kitchen L,
    2. Westmacott R,
    3. Friefeld S, et al.
    The Pediatric Stroke Outcome Measure: a validation and reliability study. Stroke. 2012;43(6):1602-1608.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. 32.↵
    1. Cleveland WS,
    2. Devlin SJ
    . Locally weighted regression: an approach to regression analysis by local fitting. J Am Stat Assoc. 1988;83(403):596-610.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  33. 33.↵
    1. Cohen J
    . Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Lawrence Elbaum Associates; 1988.
  34. 34.↵
    1. Benjamini Y,
    2. Hochberg Y
    . Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc. 1995;57(1):289-300.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. 35.↵
    1. Wickham H
    . Ggplot2 Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer; 2016.
  36. 36.↵
    1. Allman C,
    2. Scott RB
    . Neuropsychological sequelae following pediatric stroke: a nonlinear model of age at lesion effects. Child Neuropsychol. 2013;19(1):97-107.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. 37.↵
    1. Anderson V
    . Assessing executive functions in children: biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Pediatr Rehabil. 2001;4(3):119-136.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. 38.↵
    1. Cnossen MH,
    2. Aarsen FK,
    3. LjS,
    4. Renee Vandena
    . Paediatric arterial ischaemic stroke: functional outcome and risk factors.Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52(4):394-399.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. 39.↵
    1. Gogtay N,
    2. Giedd JN,
    3. Lusk L, et al.
    Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101(21):8174-8179.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. 40.↵
    1. Andersen SL
    . Trajectories of brain development: point of vulnerability or window of opportunity? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27(1-2):3-18.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. 41.↵
    1. Forbes TA,
    2. Gallo V
    . All wrapped up: environmental effects on myelination. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40(9):572-587.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  42. 42.↵
    1. Hajek CA,
    2. Yeates KO,
    3. Anderson V, et al.
    Cognitive outcomes following arterial ischemic stroke in infants and children. J Child Neurol. 2014;29(7):887-894.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. 43.↵
    1. Long B,
    2. Spencer-Smith MM,
    3. Jacobs R, et al.
    Executive function following child stroke: the impact of lesion location. J Child Neurol. 2011;26(3):279-287.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. 44.↵
    1. Grunt S,
    2. Mazenauer L,
    3. Buerki SE, et al.
    Incidence and outcomes of symptomatic neonatal arterial ischemic stroke. Pediatrics. 2015;135(5):e1220–e1228.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. 45.
    1. Brown L,
    2. Sherbenou RJ,
    3. Johnsen SK
    . TONI-4: Test of Nonverbal Intelligence, 4th ed. X.Pro-ed; 2010.
  46. 46.
    1. Petermann F,
    2. Petermann UJ
    . Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children®, 4th ed. 2012.
  47. 47.
    1. Petermann F
    . Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th ed. Pearson; 2012.
  48. 48.
    1. Delis DC,
    2. Kaplan E,
    3. Kramer J
    . Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). Pearson Psychological Corporation; 2001.
  49. 49.
    1. Helmstaedter C,
    2. Lendt M,
    3. Lux S
    . Verbaler Lern-Und Merkfähigkeitstest (VLMT). Beltz; 2001.
  50. 50.
    1. Beery KE,
    2. Beery NA,
    3. Buktenica NA
    . Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration, 6th ed. 2010.

Letters: Rapid online correspondence

No comments have been published for this article.
Comment

REQUIREMENTS

You must ensure that your Disclosures have been updated within the previous six months. Please go to our Submission Site to add or update your Disclosure information.

Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.

If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.

Submission specifications:

  • Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
  • Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
  • Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
  • Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
  • Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.

More guidelines and information on Disputes & Debates

Compose Comment

More information about text formats

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
Author Information
NOTE: The first author must also be the corresponding author of the comment.
First or given name, e.g. 'Peter'.
Your last, or family, name, e.g. 'MacMoody'.
Your email address, e.g. higgs-boson@gmail.com
Your role and/or occupation, e.g. 'Orthopedic Surgeon'.
Your organization or institution (if applicable), e.g. 'Royal Free Hospital'.
Publishing Agreement
NOTE: All authors, besides the first/corresponding author, must complete a separate Publishing Agreement Form and provide via email to the editorial office before comments can be posted.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.

Vertical Tabs

You May Also be Interested in

Back to top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Glossary
    • Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Study Funding
    • Disclosure
    • Acknowledgment
    • Appendix Authors
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Disclosures
Advertisement

Brain Metabolism Related to Mild Cognitive Impairment and Phenoconversion in Patients With Isolated REM Sleep Behavior Disorder

Dr. David E. Vaillancourt and Dr. Shannon Y. Chiu

► Watch

Related Articles

  • How Plastic Are Children?Does the Age at Stroke Occurrence Matter?

Alert Me

  • Alert me when eletters are published
Neurology: 100 (22)

Articles

  • Ahead of Print
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Popular Articles
  • Translations

About

  • About the Journals
  • Ethics Policies
  • Editors & Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertise

Submit

  • Author Center
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Information for Reviewers
  • AAN Guidelines
  • Permissions

Subscribers

  • Subscribe
  • Activate a Subscription
  • Sign up for eAlerts
  • RSS Feed
Site Logo
  • Visit neurology Template on Facebook
  • Follow neurology Template on Twitter
  • Visit Neurology on YouTube
  • Neurology
  • Neurology: Clinical Practice
  • Neurology: Education
  • Neurology: Genetics
  • Neurology: Neuroimmunology & Neuroinflammation
  • AAN.com
  • AANnews
  • Continuum
  • Brain & Life
  • Neurology Today

Wolters Kluwer Logo

Neurology | Print ISSN:0028-3878
Online ISSN:1526-632X

© 2023 American Academy of Neurology

  • Privacy Policy
  • Feedback
  • Advertise