Trajectory Pattern of Cognitive Decline in Cerebral Autosomal Dominant Arteriopathy With Subcortical Infarcts and Leukoencephalopathy
Citation Manager Formats
Make Comment
See Comments

Abstract
Background and Objectives The course and pattern of cognitive decline in ischemic cerebral small vessel disease remain poorly characterized. We analyzed the trajectory pattern of cognitive decline from age 25 to 75 years in cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL).
Methods We applied latent process mixed models to data obtained from patients with CADASIL who were repeatedly scored during their follow-up using 16 selected clinical scales or cognitive tests.
Results The modeled evolutions of these scores obtained from 1,243 observations in 265 patients recruited at the French National Referral Centre (50.1 years on average and 45.3% men) showed wide and heterogeneous variations in amplitude along the age-related progression of the disease. Although the Backward Digit Span remained essentially stable, a linear deterioration of scores obtained using the Symbol Digit Numbers or Number of Errors of Trail Making Test B was detected from 25 to 75 years. By contrast, the largest score changes were observed at midlife using the Digit Cancellation Task. All other tests related to executive functions, memory performances, or global cognitive efficiency showed a rate of change accelerating especially at the advanced stage of the disease. Male gender and the presence of gait disorders or of some disability at baseline were found to predict earlier or large changes of 4 scores (Index of Sensitivity to Cueing, Delayed Total Recall, Initiation/Perseveration, and Barthel Index) in a subgroup of individuals distinct from the rest of the sample.
Discussion Cognitive alterations develop heterogeneously during the progression of CADASIL and vary largely according to the stage of the disease. These results suggest that not only the target population and study duration but also the stage of disease progression should be considered in preparing future clinical trials aimed at reducing cognitive decline in any such condition.
Glossary
- CADASIL=
- cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy;
- cSVD=
- cerebral small vessel disease;
- EQ VAS=
- Visual Analog Scale of the 3-level EuroQol;
- IQR=
- interquartile range;
- MDRS=
- Mattis Dementia Rating Scale;
- mRS=
- modified Rankin scale;
- NIHSS=
- NIH Stroke Score;
- OR=
- odds ratio;
- TMT=
- Trail Making Test;
- VADAS-Cog=
- Vascular Dementia Assessment Scale cognitive subscale
Cerebral small vessel diseases (cSVDs) are common in the general population and represent a major source of stroke events and cognitive impairment during aging.1,2 However, despite their frequency, the natural history of cognitive decline in cSVD remains poorly understood. This is likely due to the limited duration of previous longitudinal studies and their focus on elderly individuals, in whom manifestations of vascular origin are extremely variable in severity and often intertwined with effects of cerebral aging and early neurodegenerative processes.3
Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) is a monogenic form of cerebral small artery disease caused by stereotyped mutations of the NOTCH3 gene.4 It is a unique model of severe ischemic cSVD5,6 that starts long before the effects of aging on cognition.7 Previous studies have shown that CADASIL can be responsible for slower processing speed, impaired executive functions, attention, and memory performances and, at the advanced stage, dementia.5,8,9 All these manifestations were shown to closely match those detected in sporadic forms of cSVD.6 However, the exact kinetics of cognitive decline throughout the whole course of CADASIL has not yet been investigated.
In this study, we aimed to analyze the trajectory of various cognitive deficiencies in patients with CADASIL using a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests in parallel with assessments of motor disability, functional dependence, and quality of life.
Methods
Study Population
Between 2003 and 2017, a total of 366 patients were recruited at the French National Referral Centre for rare cerebrovascular diseases, as a large prospective cohort of patients with CADASIL.10,11 Inclusion criteria were of age 18 years or older, documentation of a cysteine-related mutation in the NOTCH3 gene, and willingness to be evaluated regularly.
Standard Protocol Approvals, Registrations, and Patient Consents
Informed consent was obtained from each subject or from a close relative if necessary. The study was approved by an independent ethics committee (CEEI-IRB-17/388).
Evaluation of Patients During the Follow-up
Patients were evaluated every 18 months (on average) during the follow-up using various scales for assessing different clinical and cognitive aspects of the disease.10,11 In this study, 16 scores, determined by consensus among neurologists (N.A., D.H., and H.C.) and neuropsychologists (A.J., S.R., C.M., and C.R.) of the referral center, were selected for the analysis of global cognitive efficiency, memory performances, executive functions, focal deficits with disability, dependence, and quality of life. Ranges of the 16 scores selected for analysis in this sample are given in Table 1.
Total Number of Observations for Each Score and Number of Observations per Patient
Global cognitive efficiency was analyzed using total scores obtained from 2 measures: (1) the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS)12,13 and (2) the Vascular Dementia Assessment Scale cognitive subscale (VADAS-Cog).14 The MDRS (from 0 to 144 [best score]) includes 5 subscales related to Attention, Initiation/Perseveration, Construction, Conceptualization, and Memory.15 The VADAS-Cog (from 0 [normal] to 90 [severe impairment]) is composed of 14 items related to memory and orientation, language, praxis abilities, attention, and executive functions.14,16
Memory performance was analyzed specifically using 3 different scores obtained from the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test adapted from the Grober and Buschke procedure.17 In this study, we analyzed the capacity of retrieval or storage of information, implying correct encoding, using (1) the Total Free Recall score (without cues over the course of three 16 word recall trials varying from 0 to 48 [best score]); (2) the Index of Sensitivity to Cueing, a measure of retrieval/storage ability that decreases when information storage is compromised and cues are not useful; and (3) the Delayed Total Recall (from 0 to 16 [normal]) with both free and cued recalls.18
Executive performance, including attention and working memory performance, was analyzed using 7 different scores. Three were obtained from the VADAS-Cog: (1) the Digit Cancellation Test (from 0 [no impairment] to 10 [serious impairment]),14,19 (2) Symbol Digit Test (from 0 [no impairment] to 10 [serious impairment]), and (3) Backward Digit Span (from 0 [no impairment] to 5 [serious impairment]).14,20 The 4 other scores included (4) the Initiation/Perseveration component score from the MDRS (from 0 [worst] to 37 [best]) and 3 scores obtained from the Trail Making Test (TMT) evaluating cognitive speed and mental flexibility based on (5) the time in seconds of TMT part A, (6) the time in seconds of TMT part B, and (7) the Number of Errors in TMT part B.21,22
Disability was assessed using the modified Rankin scale (mRS). The values of 6 (death) were excluded because death was not considered in the other test scores. The severity of focal neurologic deficits accumulated over time was analyzed using the NIH Stroke Score (NIHSS).23 Daily living activities and functional independence were evaluated globally using the Barthel Index.24 Finally, quality of life was assessed using the Visual Analog Scale of the 3-level EuroQol (EQ VAS).25
Explanatory Variables
The following parameters obtained at baseline were initially considered as potential predictors of clinical worsening in CADASIL: age, sex, education, smoking, alcohol consumption, diabetes, hypertension, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, homocysteine, previous stroke events, gait disturbances, balance problems, disability, and dementia. Education was determined according to the French Barbizet scale26 split in high level (at least the final high school diploma) and low level otherwise. Alcohol consumption was a qualitative variable with 3 categories: never, <2 glasses of wine per day for a man (1 glass or equivalent for a woman), and over these thresholds. Disability was split in moderate or severe disability (mRS ≥3) and no or mild disability otherwise. Dementia was assessed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition criteria.27 Neuroimaging data were not considered in the present analysis.
Statistical Analysis
We selected from the cohort individuals having at least 1 measure for each of the 16 scores obtained during the follow-up and who were 25 years or older at inclusion and 75 years or younger during the follow-up, an age range chosen because of the small number of observations outside these 2 extremes. In cases where a subtest could not be performed because alterations were too severe, the most altered performance measure for this subtest was applied to the patient and used in the statistical analysis. In this study, only raw total scores were analyzed. Data at baseline were summarized using descriptive statistics, with frequencies and percentages for qualitative data and means and SDs for quantitative variables.
Latent process mixed models28,-,30 were used to overcome common limitations encountered during the analysis of clinical scores, particularly the frequent lack of normality in the distribution of scores, potential floor or ceiling effects, and curvilinearity.30,31 This method allowed modeling the trajectory of the 16 scores selected for analysis in a more efficient way than simpler linear mixed models.30,32 Age available throughout the follow-up was considered as the time variable for delineating the trajectories of test scores. For computation purposes, the time scale was age minus 25 years per 10 years, where 25 years corresponds to the age of the youngest patient observed at baseline. Thus, the intercept represents the cognitive level at age 25 years, and the change in cognitive level is measured for a decade.
Information on the different steps followed to build the model is given in eMethods (links.lww.com/WNL/C131). The final model was adjusted for the education level for the 12 cognitive scores.33,34 The interaction between education and time was systematically assessed using the likelihood ratio test. Because potential heterogeneity in clinical progression cannot be excluded in the study population, we also considered this possible factor in the trajectory modeling analysis.28 Without a priori, the latent process mixed models allow fitting simultaneously several models with different evolutions. Each individual is then assigned to the most probable evolution.
Finally, in cases where the evolution of a score was heterogeneous, a logistic regression model using a stepwise selection process was used to understand what distinguishes patients between groups. Multiple imputations were performed to take into account for missing data. Independent variables with p values <0.20 in the univariate setting and less than 10% missing data were introduced in the multivariate model.
Statistical tests were performed at a conventional 2-tailed type I error of 0.05. Data were analyzed using SAS software version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) and R software version 3.6.1.35 Latent process mixed models and latent class mixed models were estimated using the package “lcmm” version 1.8.1.36
Data Availability
Data not provided in the article because of space limitations may be shared (anonymized) at the request of any qualified investigator for purposes of replicating procedures and results.
Results
Main Characteristics
The study sample consisted of a total of 1,243 observations obtained from 265 patients across a median number of 4 visits per individual (interquartile range [IQR]: 2–7 and eTable1, links.lww.com/WNL/C131). The median number of observations per patient ranged from 3 for the VADAS-Cog Total score to 6 for the NIHSS, Barthel Index, and mRS scores (Table 1). The follow-up duration varied from 0 to 14 years, with a median follow-up interval of 17 months (IQR: 9–23).
The 265 patients included in the analysis were younger, more educated, and more frequently never-smokers than the cohort participants who were not included in the analysis (Table 2). In addition, patients included in the analysis were less severely affected than those who were not included in the analysis, presenting less frequently with a history of stroke events, gait disturbances, balance problems, disability, dementia at inclusion, and better scores in all tests and scales except for the number of near-zero TMT B Errors (Table 2).
Baseline Characteristics of Patients Included in the Analysis and Comparison With Cohort Individuals Who Were Not Included
Trajectories of Cognitive and Associated Clinical Scores
Using different link functions (eTable 2, links.lww.com/WNL/C131), we modeled the trajectory of the latent process underlying each score with a linear (y = β1x + β0), quadratic (β2x2 + β1x + β0), or cubic (y = β3x3 + β2x2 + β1x + β0) form. The best model is given in eTable 3. We then used the link function to convert predictions obtained in the latent process scale to trajectories of all scores in their natural scale. These trajectories are illustrated for the different tests in Figures 1 and 2 (the corresponding underlying raw data are not shown for readability).
Score trajectories showing homogeneous changes among patients from 25 to 75 years (according to the best latent process model with corresponding 95% confidence intervals (in gray) and after adjustment for the education level for all cognitive tests). The analysis of cognitive scores was performed after adjustment for the education level at baseline. No significant interaction was detected between education level and time for any score except TMT B Time, which increased later and more steeply after 55 years of age in patients with a high education level. Vertical lines indicate where data were calculated from the model at limits of 3 age-reference intervals.
Score trajectories delineated by the model showing a group of individuals with worst (or earlier) deterioration distinct from the rest of the population. Two groups were identified with modeling trajectories for the Index of Sensitivity to Cueing, Delayed Total Recall, Initiation/Perseveration subscore, and Barthel Index: a group with early or larger changes (in red), and a group with late or limited changes (in blue). The number of participants in each group is detailed for each score. The analysis of cognitive scores was performed after adjustment for the education level at baseline. Vertical lines indicate where results were obtained using the model at the limits of age tertiles.
Homogeneous Evolution of 12 Cognitive and Clinical Scores
For 9 cognitive scores, the best latent process model selected included only a single latent class: Total Free Recall, Digit Cancellation Task (correct numbers crossed off), Symbol Digit Correct Numbers, Backward Digit Span, TMT A Time, TMT B Errors, TMT B Time, MDRS Total score, and VADAS-Cog Total score. The different trajectories obtained after adjustment for education are shown in Figure 1. In all corresponding models, no significant interaction was detected between education and time except for TMT B Time, for which a steeper and later progression was detected in patients with a high education level (Figure 1F). For 3 clinical scores (NIHSS, mRS, and EQ VAS), the best latent process model selected also included only a single latent class (Figure 1, J–L).
Heterogeneous Evolution of 4 Cognitive and Clinical Scores
For 3 cognitive scores—Index of Sensitivity to Cueing, Delayed Total Recall, and Initiation/Perseveration subscore of the MDRS—the best model for score changes included 2 latent classes (eAppendix1, links.lww.com/WNL/C131) with no significant interaction between education and time (Figure 2). The Index of Sensitivity to Cueing decreased early in 46 patients and late in 219 patients, whereas the Initiation/Perseveration subscore of the MDRS decreased early in 60 patients and late in 205 patients. By contrast, the Delayed Total Recall score decreased in 54 patients but remained stable in 211 patients. The Barthel Index was best modeled with 2 class-specific predicted trajectories with a strong early decrease in scores in 22 patients and a much later decrease in 243 patients.
The variables associated with these different classes of evolution in univariate analysis are detailed in Table 3 and with the data after imputation of missing data (eTable 4, links.lww.com/WNL/C131). As the results were similar, multivariate models were performed with the data without imputation. The final multivariate model showed the following: (1) an earlier decline in the Index of Sensitivity to Cueing was only associated with male sex (odds ratio [OR] 4.78, 95% CI 2.30–9.94, p < 0.001); (2) the decline in the Delayed Total Recall score was associated with male sex (OR 1.04, 95% CI 1.00–1.07, p = 0.049), age (OR 4.55 per additional year, 95% CI 2.22–9.34, p < 0.001), and the presence of gait disturbances (OR 3.50, 95% CI 1.55–7.92, p = 0.003) at inclusion, while highly educated participants were less likely to belong to this group (OR 0.45, 95% CI 0.22–0.93, p = 0.030); and (3) the earlier change in the Initiation/Perseveration subscore of the MDRS was independently related to male sex (OR 3.22, 95% CI 1.60–6.45, p = 0.001), moderate or severe disability (OR 15.68, 95% CI 3.10–79.33, p = 0.001), and higher MADRS scores (OR 1.06 per unit increase, 95% CI 1.02–1.11, p = 0.007) at inclusion; participants with a high level of education were also less likely to belong to this early decline group (OR 0.39, 95% CI 0.20–0.79, p = 0.009). For the Barthel Index, the multivariate analysis showed that participants with gait disturbances (OR 13.18, 95% CI 3.96–43.80, p < 0.001) and those with moderate or severe disability (OR 19.02, 95% CI 3.69–97.96, p < 0.001) at inclusion were more likely to belong to the earliest decline group.
Baseline Characteristics Predicting an Earlier or Larger Deterioration for 4 Scores (Index of Sensitivity to Cueing, Delayed Total Recall, Initiation/Perseveration Subscore, and Barthel Index) in a Subgroup of Patients (Univariate Logistic Regressions)
Modeling Trajectories Revealed Early, Intermediate, and Late Score Changes in Variable Amplitude
To facilitate the translation of modeled trajectories to clinical practice, we also calculated the cognitive score changes during the follow-up depicted in Figures 1 and 2 using the latent process model at the age limits of 3 age-related reference intervals −25 to 41.7, 41.7 to 58.3, and 58.3–75 years of age-derived from the age range of the study sample (Tables 4 and 5). A significant score change was detected between each of the pairwise age groups for the Symbol Digit Correct Numbers. Most other cognitive performances significantly deteriorated over both the second and third age intervals with much larger changes over the interval of 58.3–75 years for Total Free Recall, TMT A Time, TMT B Time, MDRS Total score, VADAS-Cog Total score, Index of Sensitivity to Cueing (in group 2), and Initiation/Perseveration subscore of the MDRS (in groups 1 and 2) except for the Digit Cancellation Crossed Off Task, which showed much larger changes between 41.7 and 58.3 years (Tables 4 and 5).
Variations Estimated by the Latent-Process Model at the Limits of Age Tertiles for Scores With Homogeneous Evolution
Variations Estimated by the Latent-Process Model at the Limits of Age Tertiles for Scores With Heterogeneous Evolution (A Separate Group of Patients Presented With Earlier or Larger Score Changes [Group 1] Than the Rest of the Sample [Group 2])
Variable changes over time were also detected for other global clinical measures. Changes in the global disability assessed using the mRS, the NIHSS, and the Barthel Index (groups 1 and 2) were found mainly during intermediate-age and late-age intervals. Conversely, changes in the quality of life, measured by EQ VAS scores, occurred from the early stage of the disease onward.
Discussion
The results of this study show that cognitive decline not only develops gradually over several decades but also heterogeneously and depends on the cognitive ability evaluated and test used in CADASIL. Modeled evolution curves of different test scores vary widely in global amplitude and according to age. Although the Backward Digit Span remained essentially stable, a linear deterioration of scores obtained using the Symbol Digit Numbers or Number of Errors of TMT B was detected from 25 to 75 years. Conversely, changes observed using the Digit Cancellation Task show the largest variation occurring at midlife, with little or no significant change observed at the youngest or oldest ages. The progression of the other measures related to executive functions, memory performance, or global cognitive efficiency shows little or no variation at the onset of the disease, followed by an increase in changes that accelerates with aging.
Analysis of modeled curves allowed us to predict the potential extent of score changes over 3 age reference groups. Between 25 and 41.7 years, the first score to worsen is the Symbol Digit Number score, which primarily measures the speed of information processing.37 In line, a trend toward an increase in TMT B Time by 30 seconds is also predicted over the same age interval in individuals with a low education level. The lack of significant results in highly educated patients might be related to the strong influence of education and practice effects on some aspects of cognitive processing speed.38 Using a simple computer interface, we previously highlighted an increased reaction time for visual stimuli in patients with CADASIL for whom global cognitive abilities were preserved.39 All these results converge on the hypothesis that cognitive processing can long exhibit an isolated and limited slowdown at the onset of the disease.
By contrast, between 41.7 and 58.3 years of age, the model predicts that Symbol Digit Number scores continue to deteriorate at the same rate but in combination with alterations of other cognitive skills. The large increase in time to completion of TMT A and B during this age interval is possibly related to the further increase of cognitive slowness.22 The largest changes are detected using the Digit Cancellation Test which evaluates both focused and selective attention.19 This is observed in parallel with a reduction in the Initiation/Perseveration subscore, which mainly reflects a reduction in mental flexibility. These results are consistent because attentional control plays a key role in cognitive flexibility, defined as the ability to switch between different mental operations and strategies and to simultaneously override habitual responses.38 The high effect of education on cognitive flexibility also explains the largest change in the Initiation/Perseveration subscore in our patients having the lowest education level.40 In the same age interval, alterations in memory performance become detectable. The Total Free Recall is reduced, while the Index of Sensitivity to Cueing is only slightly decreased, and Delayed Total Recall is globally maintained. This indicates that memory retrieval of information is primarily affected, while the encoding of information remains largely preserved. Alterations in attention and mental flexibility might also be involved at this level.41 Finally, at this stage, the decreases in VADAS-Cog and MDRS scores may simply result from all previous specific alterations leading to a global reduction in cognitive efficiency. This occurs when some motor disability and functional alterations are just appearing, but while most patients remain independent.
After the age of 57.3 years, the model shows a steep acceleration of cognitive decline and disability. All chronometric measures related to mental processing speed, attention, and executive dysfunction showed the greatest increase. Memory performances and global efficiency scores drop sharply. Significant changes in TMT B accuracy also occur at this stage, possibly reflecting severe frontal dysfunction.42,43 Surprisingly, no significant change in the Backward Digit Span score is predicted by the model. This test has been previously linked to working memory and planning ability, as well as visual image processing.44,45 Our results may indicate that the underlying cognitive processes are essentially preserved during disease progression, but a poor sensitivity of the test to moderate working memory changes cannot be excluded.
Four measures, the Initiation/Perseveration subscale, Index of Sensitivity to Cueing, Delayed Total Recall and Barthel Index, do not follow a homogeneous progression across the entire study sample. For these tests, a group of individuals present with an earlier or larger decline in performance compared with the others. Depending on the test, these groups are of variable size, ranging from 22 to 60 individuals, half of whom belonged to another group (data not shown). This separation into distinct groups is possibly related to some intrinsic properties of each corresponding measure. Thus, the difference detected between the heterogeneous course of the Initiation/Perseveration subscore of the MDRS compared with the homogeneous variation in the global MDRS score illustrates that some specific cognitive changes occurring early in a distinct group of patients might be masked by a more global measure. These results may also support that some individuals within the study population really have a different evolution profile and may worsen much more rapidly than the other participants on some specific clinical aspects.
The results of this study are important for designing future clinical trials in ischemic cSVD, and particularly in CADASIL. Because clinical trials can be performed over a limited time frame, most likely less than 2 or 3 years, the present results support the conclusion that outcome metrics for measuring cognitive or clinical changes should be carefully selected during such a limited period. Our study suggests that, during the first 2 decades overall after the age of 25 years, only measures highly sensitive to processing speed should be considered. At this stage, only white-matter lesions are usually detected on cerebral MRI in patients with CADASIL.5,46,47 Their location within the forceps minor or along the thalamofrontal tracts might be responsible for cognitive slowdown.48 In this study, only limited variations are detected using the Symbol Digit Test. Additional studies are thus needed to determine which tests should be used for capturing the limited changes in cognitive processing speed when white-matter lesions are just appearing or expand slowly. Later, over the next 2 decades, the choice of cognitive tests capable of capturing variations over 2–3 years seems easier. Our results suggest that measures sensitive to cognitive speed, attention, and executive dysfunction should be used. Chronometric measures showing the largest changes seem to be of particular interest. A combination of cognitive measures may also represent an alternative approach for capturing the larger spectrum of cognitive alterations. The VADAS-Cog Total score seems a promising approach.14 An average variation of 9 points is predicted by our model over 17 years. Conversely, the modeled variation of the MDRS score, which is limited to 4 points over the same age interval, does not seem to be of sufficient amplitude to be recommended as a unique outcome. Finally, at the latest stage of the disease, the same tools, including the MDRS, could be proposed because their largest modifications are detected after the fifth decade.
Our results also suggest that some predictors may help identify patients with the highest risk of cognitive or clinical changes, particularly when memory performance, executive dysfunction, or functional dependence are targeted. Patients with some initial manifestations of disability or gait alterations might be at the highest risk of rapid cognitive decline, as previously reported in a 3-year longitudinal study.11 In contrast to all other cognitive or clinical test measures, the EuroQol score decreases progressively and linearly from 25 years, which does not seem related to changes in disability or cognitive scores. The multidimensional nature of quality of life, which includes various psychological, social, cultural, and economic aspects in addition to health status, might explain this discrepancy.25,49
This study has a number of limitations. Our study sample does not represent the entire population of patients with CADASIL. Our cohort participants were selected from our National Referral Centre, which only includes volunteers who can travel. Moreover, only individuals who had at least 1 measure for each of the 16 scores during the follow-up were included in the analysis. Consequently, patients at the most severe clinical stage who could not be assessed repeatedly are most likely underrepresented. On the other hand, we think that our sample is conversely well representative of the individuals who might be included in future therapeutic trials. The exclusion of incomplete data from analysis represents another potential selection bias. However, our results did not differ after including all data collected and using multiple imputations for lacking results. Another potential limitation is that, as age was considered as the time scale, this study is unable to discriminate effects related to aging from those related to the disease itself as often reported in studies of long-term degenerative disorders. Unfortunately, age at clinical onset is also particularly difficult to determine in this condition which may start by attacks of migraine with aura which are inconsistent and unrelated to the clinical severity or by subtle cognitive impairment particularly difficult to identify.
The study also has multiple strengths. The amount of data is considerable, particularly for such a rare disease. The neuropsychological measures assessed through a large battery of tests were selected by consensus as those that best reflect the most salient cognitive features of the disease. Our data were also acquired in multiple individuals of different ages and at very different stages of the disease. Thus, their evaluation at multiple time points allows mimicking a continuous collection of information. Finally, latent process modeling used to delineate their trajectories is a robust method considering missing at random data.
Finally, we think that these long-term cognitive changes delineated in CADASIL may indicate that the tools used to assess cognitive decline in similar conditions must be chosen with extreme caution depending on the rate of progression of the underlying disease, the duration of the study, and the stage of disease progression.
Study Funding
This research was supported by the ANR grant: RHU TRT_cSVD (ANR:16-RHUS-0004) and also by the Association ARNEVA (Association de Recherche en Neurologie Vasculaire).
Disclosure
The authors report no relevant disclosures. Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures.
Acknowledgment
The authors warmly thank the team in charge of formatting and cleaning the database using multiple information collected over 14 years from different sources and which made possible this study, particularly Prof. Sylvie Chevret and Mrs. Claire Pacheco (INSERM UMR1136). The authors thank very much Mr. Abbas Taleb responsible for collecting most of information along the cohort study, Mrs. Solange Hello who managed and organized the appointment of multiple family members involved in the study, Mrs. Jocelyne Ruffie, Mrs. Annie Kurtz, and Prof. M.G. Bousser who had a crucial role when we initiated the collection of these data. The authors also thank the CADASIL France Association for their help and permanent support.
Appendix 1 Authors

Footnotes
Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article.
↵* These authors contributed equally to this work as senior authors.
The Article Processing Charge was funded by the authors.
Submitted and externally peer reviewed. The handling editor was Linda Hershey, MD, PhD, FAAN.
- Received January 3, 2022.
- Accepted in final form April 14, 2022.
- Copyright © 2022 The Author(s). Published by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. on behalf of the American Academy of Neurology.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND), which permits downloading and sharing the work provided it is properly cited. The work cannot be changed in any way or used commercially without permission from the journal.
References
- 1.↵
- 2.↵
- 3.↵
- 4.↵
- 5.↵
- Chabriat H,
- Joutel A,
- Tournier-Lasserve E,
- Bousser MG
- 6.↵
- Charlton RA,
- Morris RG,
- Nitkunan A,
- Markus HS
- 7.↵
- 8.↵
- 9.↵
- 10.↵
- 11.↵
- Chabriat H,
- Hervé D,
- Duering M, et al
- 12.↵
- 13.↵
- 14.↵
- 15.↵
- 16.↵
- 17.↵
- Grober E,
- Lipton RB,
- Hall C,
- Crystal H
- 18.↵
- 19.↵
- Hatta T,
- Yoshizaki K,
- Mase M,
- Ito Y,
- Kabasawa H
- 20.↵
- 21.↵
- 22.↵
- 23.↵
- Adams HP,
- Davis PH,
- Leira EC, et al
- 24.↵
- 25.↵
- 26.↵
- Barbizet J
- 27.↵American Psychological Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. APA, 1994.
- 28.↵
- 29.↵
- 30.↵
- 31.↵
- 32.↵
- Brice S,
- Jabouley A,
- Reyes S, et al
- 33.↵
- 34.↵
- 35.↵R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2018.
- 36.↵
- Proust-Lima C,
- Philipps V,
- Liquet B
- 37.↵
- 38.↵
- MacPherson SE,
- Cox SR,
- Dickie DA, et al
- 39.↵
- Jouvent E,
- Reyes S,
- De Guio F,
- Chabriat H
- 40.↵
- Delgado-Losada ML,
- Rubio-Valdehita S,
- Lopez-Higes R, et al
- 41.↵
- Madore KP,
- Khazenzon AM,
- Backes CW, et al
- 42.↵
- 43.↵
- Christidi F,
- Kararizou E,
- Triantafyllou NI,
- Paraskevas GP,
- Zalonis I
- 44.↵
- Gregoire J,
- Van Der Linden M
- 45.↵
- 46.↵
- 47.↵
- Chabriat H,
- Levy C,
- Taillia H, et al
- 48.↵
- 49.↵
Letters: Rapid online correspondence
REQUIREMENTS
If you are uploading a letter concerning an article:
You must have updated your disclosures within six months: http://submit.neurology.org
Your co-authors must send a completed Publishing Agreement Form to Neurology Staff (not necessary for the lead/corresponding author as the form below will suffice) before you upload your comment.
If you are responding to a comment that was written about an article you originally authored:
You (and co-authors) do not need to fill out forms or check disclosures as author forms are still valid
and apply to letter.
Submission specifications:
- Submissions must be < 200 words with < 5 references. Reference 1 must be the article on which you are commenting.
- Submissions should not have more than 5 authors. (Exception: original author replies can include all original authors of the article)
- Submit only on articles published within 6 months of issue date.
- Do not be redundant. Read any comments already posted on the article prior to submission.
- Submitted comments are subject to editing and editor review prior to posting.
You May Also be Interested in
Hemiplegic Migraine Associated With PRRT2 Variations A Clinical and Genetic Study
Dr. Robert Shapiro and Dr. Amynah Pradhan